全文获取类型
收费全文 | 183篇 |
免费 | 2篇 |
国内免费 | 1篇 |
专业分类
186篇 |
出版年
2022年 | 1篇 |
2021年 | 3篇 |
2020年 | 1篇 |
2019年 | 1篇 |
2018年 | 7篇 |
2017年 | 7篇 |
2016年 | 7篇 |
2015年 | 6篇 |
2014年 | 5篇 |
2013年 | 11篇 |
2012年 | 3篇 |
2011年 | 6篇 |
2010年 | 7篇 |
2009年 | 5篇 |
2008年 | 9篇 |
2007年 | 2篇 |
2006年 | 5篇 |
2005年 | 30篇 |
2004年 | 20篇 |
2003年 | 12篇 |
2002年 | 3篇 |
2001年 | 5篇 |
1999年 | 2篇 |
1998年 | 1篇 |
1997年 | 2篇 |
1996年 | 1篇 |
1995年 | 3篇 |
1993年 | 1篇 |
1992年 | 8篇 |
1991年 | 1篇 |
1990年 | 1篇 |
1989年 | 1篇 |
1988年 | 1篇 |
1987年 | 2篇 |
1984年 | 3篇 |
1982年 | 1篇 |
1980年 | 1篇 |
1972年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有186条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
11.
Donald G. York 《Zygon》1995,30(3):477-478
Abstract. Frank Tipler's The Physics of Immortality provides abundant cause for intellectual offense—including challenges to physics, to theology, and, seemingly, to common sense. Few philosophical conundrums remain unaddressed. Still, the book is stimulating and well presented. 相似文献
12.
13.
14.
Domestic chicks are capable of perceiving as a whole objects partly concealed by occluders (amodal completion). In previous studies chicks were imprinted on a certain configuration and at test they were required to choose between two alternative versions of it. Using the same paradigm we now investigated the presence of hemispheric differences in amodal completion by testing newborn chicks with one eye temporarily patched. Separate groups of newly hatched chicks were imprinted binocularly: (1) on a square partly occluded by a superimposed bar, (2) on a whole or (3) on an amputated version of the square. At test, in monocular conditions, each chick was presented with a free choice between a complete and an amputated square. In the crucial condition 1, chicks tested with only their left eye in use chose the complete square (like binocular chicks would do); right-eyed chicks, in contrast, tended to choose the amputated square. Similar results were obtained in another group of chicks imprinted binocularly onto a cross (either occluded or amputated in its central part) and required to choose between a complete or an amputated cross. Left-eyed and binocular chicks chose the complete cross, whereas right-eyed chicks did not choose the amputated cross significantly more often. These findings suggest that neural structures fed by the left eye (mainly located in the right hemisphere) are, in the chick, more inclined to a global analysis of visual scenes, whereas those fed by the right eye seem to be more inclined to a featural analysis of visual scenes. 相似文献
15.
We report experiments based on a novel test in domestic chicks (Gallus gallus), designed to examine the encoding of two different geometric features of an enclosed environment: relative lengths of the walls and amplitude of the corners. Chicks were trained to search for a food reward located in one corner of a parallelogram-shaped enclosure. Between trials, chicks were passively disoriented and the enclosure was rotated, making reorientation possible only on the basis of the internal spatial structure of the enclosure. In order to reorient, chicks could rely on two sources of information: the relative lengths of the walls of the enclosure (associated to their left-right sense order) and the angles subtended by walls at corners. Chicks learned the task choosing equally often the reinforced corner and its rotational equivalent. Results of tests carried out in novel enclosures, the shapes of which were chosen ad hoc (1) to induce reorientation based only on the ratio of walls lengths plus sense (rectangular enclosure), or (2) to induce reorientation based only on corner angles (rhombus-shaped enclosure), suggested that chicks encoded both features of the environment. In a third test, in which chicks faced a conflict between these geometric features (mirror parallelogram-shaped enclosure), reorientation seemed to depend on the salience of corner angles. These results shed light on the elements of the environmental geometry which control spatial reorientation, and broaden the knowledge on the geometric representation of space in animals. 相似文献
16.
A necessary and sufficient condition is given in this paper for the existence and uniqueness of the maximum likelihood (the
so-called joint maximum likelihood) estimate of the parameters of the Partial Credit Model. This condition is stated in terms
of a structural property of the pattern of the data matrix that can be easily verified on the basis of a simple iterative
procedure. The result is proved by using an argument of Haberman (1977).
The author wishes to thank the Editor and the anonymous reviewers for their comments that helped to substantially improve
the final version of this paper.
This research was supported in part by a MURST grant (ex 60%). 相似文献
17.
In the tradition of substructural logics, it has been claimed for a long time that conjunction and inclusive disjunction are ambiguous:we should, in fact, distinguish between ‘lattice’ connectives (also called additive or extensional) and ‘group’ connectives (also called multiplicative or intensional). We argue that an analogous ambiguity affects the quantifiers. Moreover, we show how such a perspective could yield solutions for two well-known logical puzzles: McGee’s counterexample to modus ponens and the lottery paradox. 相似文献
18.
19.
We present a new database of Dutch word frequencies based on film and television subtitles, and we validate it with a lexical
decision study involving 14,000 monosyllabic and disyllabic Dutch words. The new SUBTLEX frequencies explain up to 10% more
variance in accuracies and reaction times (RTs) of the lexical decision task than the existing CELEX word frequency norms,
which are based largely on edited texts. As is the case for English, an accessibility measure based on contextual diversity
explains more of the variance in accuracy and RT than does the raw frequency of occurrence counts. The database is freely
available for research purposes and may be downloaded from the authors’ university site at http://crr.ugent.be/subtlex-nl
or from http://brm psychonomic-journals.org/content/supplemental. 相似文献
20.
Walter R. Schumm A. Diane Ferguson Malika S. Hashmat Telisa L. New 《Islam & Christian-Muslim Relations》2005,16(2):167-185
Historically Islam has been recognized as a religion that is logical. Christianity has long been recognized as having a number of beliefs or doctrines that could be described as paradoxical. Sixty-nine religious doctrines or beliefs were evaluated for paradoxical content and in terms of whether Islam and Christianity agreed or disagreed with respect to each doctrine or belief. It was hypothesized that disagreement between the two religions would be much more common with respect to paradoxical doctrines or beliefs. Nearly 90% of doctrinal agreement or disagreement could be traced to the paradoxical or non-paradoxical content of the beliefs evaluated. The relationship between agreement and paradox was very significant statistically. Implications for future Christian–Muslim dialogue are discussed. 相似文献