全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1761篇 |
免费 | 24篇 |
专业分类
1785篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 12篇 |
2023年 | 16篇 |
2022年 | 15篇 |
2021年 | 19篇 |
2020年 | 40篇 |
2019年 | 56篇 |
2018年 | 80篇 |
2017年 | 74篇 |
2016年 | 68篇 |
2015年 | 40篇 |
2014年 | 68篇 |
2013年 | 168篇 |
2012年 | 92篇 |
2011年 | 81篇 |
2010年 | 47篇 |
2009年 | 50篇 |
2008年 | 74篇 |
2007年 | 81篇 |
2006年 | 55篇 |
2005年 | 49篇 |
2004年 | 53篇 |
2003年 | 58篇 |
2002年 | 66篇 |
2001年 | 35篇 |
2000年 | 31篇 |
1999年 | 26篇 |
1998年 | 26篇 |
1997年 | 15篇 |
1996年 | 20篇 |
1995年 | 18篇 |
1994年 | 19篇 |
1993年 | 8篇 |
1992年 | 19篇 |
1991年 | 13篇 |
1990年 | 14篇 |
1989年 | 13篇 |
1988年 | 21篇 |
1987年 | 11篇 |
1986年 | 11篇 |
1985年 | 11篇 |
1984年 | 11篇 |
1983年 | 6篇 |
1982年 | 8篇 |
1981年 | 7篇 |
1980年 | 9篇 |
1979年 | 7篇 |
1978年 | 12篇 |
1976年 | 9篇 |
1972年 | 5篇 |
1969年 | 7篇 |
排序方式: 共有1785条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
811.
812.
Kelly KM Shedlosky-Shoemaker R Porter K Remy A DeSimone P Andrykowski MA 《Journal of genetic counseling》2007,16(3):373-382
Family history is one the greatest risk factors for disease and one of the most important informational tools in medical genetics
for the purpose of diagnosis, risk assessment, prevention and treatment. However, research is needed on the comparability
of different methods of cancer family history assessment and the influence of psychosocial factors in family history reports.
The purpose of this study was to determine if individuals had discrepancies between written and interview reports of cancer
family history and the role of psychosocial factors in these discrepancies. Oncology patients (n=104) were administered a survey to assess psychosocial factors (i.e., information-seeking, worry, perceived risk, and health
literacy) and were asked to provide family history in a written and an interview form. Randomization determined which form
individuals received first. No differences in the amount of missing data or the amount of unspecified data were noted between
the written and interview method. Psychosocial factors did not differentiate between those who had discrepancies in family
history reports and those who did not have discrepancies in family history reports; although there was a trend for those with
lower literacy and those who were blunters to be more discrepant on type of cancer diagnosis. In sum, this preliminary study
indicates that written and interview methods of family history assessment for first degree relatives may be used interchangeably.
The ability to use written methods will facilitate collection of basic family history information in the oncology clinic. 相似文献
813.
The present study evaluated sex differences in observational fear conditioning using modeled “mock” panic attacks as an unconditioned stimulus (UCS). Fifty-nine carefully prescreened healthy undergraduate participants (30 women) underwent 3 consecutive differential conditioning phases: habituation, acquisition, and extinction. It was expected that participants watching a confederate display mock panic attacks (UCS) paired with a previously neutral stimulus (CS+) would learn to respond fearfully to the CS+, but not to the CS− (i.e., a stimulus never associated with displays of panic). Women also were expected to report more distress and ratings of panic to the CS+ than the CS− compared to men, but no sex differences were anticipated on autonomic indices of conditioning (i.e., electrodermal responses). Consistent with expectation, aversive conditioning was demonstrated by greater magnitude electrodermal and verbal-evaluative (e.g., subjective units of distress scale, panic ratings) responses to the CS+ over the CS−, with women reporting more distress to the CS+ over the CS−, but not greater autonomic conditioning, compared to men. Overall, the results support the notion that modeled panic attacks can serve as a potent UCS for both men and women. Discussion focuses on sex differences in observational fear conditioning and its relation to the clinical presentation of anxiety disorders. 相似文献
814.
Learning a new motor skill can induce neuronal plasticity in rats. Within motor cortex, learning-induced plasticity includes dendritic reorganization, synaptogenesis, and changes in synapse morphology. Behavioral studies have demonstrated that learning requires protein synthesis. It is likely that some of the proteins synthesized during learning are involved in, or the result of, learning-induced structural plasticity. We predicted the expression of proteins involved in neural plasticity would be altered in a learning dependent fashion. Long-Evans rats were trained on a series of motor tasks that varied in complexity, so that the effects of activity could be teased apart from the effects of learning. The motor cortices were examined for MAP2 and synaptophysin protein using Western blotting and immunohistochemistry. Western blotting revealed that expression of MAP2 was not detectably influenced by learning, whereas synaptophysin expression increased on day 1, 3, and 5 of complex motor skill learning. Expression of MAP2 does not seem to indicate difficulty of task or duration of training time, whereas increases in synaptophysin expression, which appear diffusely across the cortex, seem to be correlated with the first 5 days of motor skill learning. Similar findings with GAP-43 suggest the change in synaptophysin may coincide with synapse formation. Immunohistochemistry did not reveal any localized changes in protein expression. These data indicate a difference in learning-induced expression in the mammalian brain compared to reports in the literature, which have often focused on stimulation to induce alterations in protein expression. 相似文献
815.
816.
817.
ABSTRACT When is it right to enforce medical treatment on a patient who is refusing that treatment? English law recognises two ethical principles as of paramount importance: the autonomy of the patient; and the consequences of not treating compared with treating. The law, by and large, operates these principles in succession. Thus, in the case of a patient refusing treatment, the law asks first, is the patient competent? Only if the answer is no, are the consequences considered. We criticise the position taken by English law and argue, first, that competence is a graded and not a binary concept, and secondly, that the two ethical principles should be applied not sequentially but at the same time. These two ideas form what we have called the balance model. This model could be used for an empirical study of individuals’ ethical beliefs, and in particular to test the hypothesis that the ethical beliefs of most individuals conform to the balance model rather than to the position taken by English law. 相似文献
818.
What is it Like to be a Phenomenologist? 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
In a recent paper, 'On the Persistence of Phenomenology' (in T. Metzinger (ed.), Conscious Experience , Paderborn, 1995), Diana Raffman presents a new argument for qualia, an argument that provides new focus for the qualia debate. We think Raffman's work relocates the debate to a better neighbourhood, a neighbourhood in which what tempts us to think that there must be qualia or phenomenal information is highlighted. Raffman, we argue, locates the core thought shared by many of the friends of qualia, namely, that our conceptual resources are inadequate for capturing the richness of experience. Experience is ineffably determinate. Moreover, Raffman's argument in support of this thought seems especially embarrassing to scientific-minded materialists, since the argument relies largely on well established empirical facts. We show, however, that Raffman's argument fails. So, if we are correct that her argument highlights what is tempting about qualia, then we have also shown that it is not tempting. 相似文献
819.
Coping, Conflictual Social Interactions, Social Support, and Mood Among HIV-Infected Persons 总被引:7,自引:0,他引:7
Fleishman JA Sherbourne CD Crystal S Collins RL Marshall GN Kelly M Bozzette SA Shapiro MF Hays RD 《American journal of community psychology》2000,28(4):421-453
This study considers the interrelationships among coping, conflictual social interactions, and social support, as well as their combined associations with positive and negative mood. Research has shown that each of these variables affects adjustment to stressful circumstances. Few studies, however, examine this full set of variables simultaneously. One hundred forty HIV-infected persons completed a questionnaire containing measures of coping, social support, conflictual social interactions, and positive and negative mood. Factor analyses showed that perceived social support and conflictual social interactions formed separate factors and were not strongly related. Compared to perceived social support, social conflict was more strongly related to coping behaviors, especially to social isolation, anger, and wishful thinking. Conflictual social interactions were more strongly related to negative mood than was perceived social support. Coping by withdrawing socially was significantly related to less positive and greater negative mood. The findings point to the importance of simultaneously considering coping, supportive relationships, and conflictual relationships in studies of adjustment to chronic illness. In particular, a dynamic may occur in which conflictual social interactions and social isolation aggravate each other and result in escalating psychological distress. 相似文献
820.