全文获取类型
收费全文 | 537篇 |
免费 | 49篇 |
国内免费 | 159篇 |
专业分类
745篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 1篇 |
2023年 | 9篇 |
2022年 | 13篇 |
2021年 | 16篇 |
2020年 | 20篇 |
2019年 | 14篇 |
2018年 | 21篇 |
2017年 | 30篇 |
2016年 | 27篇 |
2015年 | 12篇 |
2014年 | 16篇 |
2013年 | 43篇 |
2012年 | 20篇 |
2011年 | 53篇 |
2010年 | 32篇 |
2009年 | 37篇 |
2008年 | 41篇 |
2007年 | 47篇 |
2006年 | 64篇 |
2005年 | 41篇 |
2004年 | 24篇 |
2003年 | 17篇 |
2002年 | 17篇 |
2001年 | 14篇 |
2000年 | 6篇 |
1999年 | 14篇 |
1998年 | 7篇 |
1997年 | 18篇 |
1996年 | 11篇 |
1995年 | 13篇 |
1994年 | 5篇 |
1993年 | 3篇 |
1992年 | 5篇 |
1991年 | 4篇 |
1990年 | 4篇 |
1989年 | 5篇 |
1988年 | 1篇 |
1987年 | 5篇 |
1986年 | 3篇 |
1985年 | 1篇 |
1983年 | 2篇 |
1982年 | 4篇 |
1981年 | 2篇 |
1979年 | 3篇 |
排序方式: 共有745条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
71.
初中学生道德推理、移情反应、亲社会行为及其相互关系的比较研究 总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3
抽取上海市初中三个年级217名学生为被试,通过对道德判断推理能力、移情反应、亲社会行为的测量和问卷调查,考察初中阶段学生在这三个方面的发展规律,以及三者之间的相互关系。结果显示:道德判断推理与亲社会行为、与移情能力之问没有显著相关;移情能力中的个别指标与亲社会行为之间相关显著;在移情反应、亲社会行为水平、以及助人动机、帮助方式等方面均存在显著的性别差异;故事呈现方式不同会对故事与自己相关程度产生影响。 相似文献
72.
2008年2月2日,中共中央统战部、全国政协民宗委、国家宗教事务局共同主办的"2008年宗教界人士迎春茶话会"在国家宗教事务局银安殿举行.中共中央统战部常务副部长朱维群,全国政协民宗委副主任黄璜,全国政协副秘书长蒋作君,国家宗教事务局局长叶小文、副局长王作安、齐晓飞、蒋坚永等有关方面领导和五大宗教全国性团体负责人近百人出席了茶活会. 相似文献
73.
快速启动范式(Sereno & Rayner, 1992)是一种基于呈现随眼动变化技术而设计的眼动范式,主要用于研究中央凹视野内字词识别的瞬时启动,通过精确控制启动条件和启动时窗,以考察词汇通达过程中词汇水平(如,词频、词长等)、亚词汇水平(如,语音、语义、字形等)信息的加工时序。本文从基本原理、设计基础、应用领域、注意事项等方面对快速启动范式进行了较为深入的解读和介绍,并结合已有的研究成果,指出了该范式在汉语阅读中的应用价值。 相似文献
74.
近代福建,天主教区、华人基督徒、新教传教士分别参与、发动和组织了当地天主教徒、基督教徒移民非洲、东南亚地区的集体移民活动.本文尝试结合基督教研究与华侨华人史研究,对这三次集体移民做一述评,分析比较移民活动参与者、组织者及其社会角色的异同.文章认为,近代基督宗教在华人移民海外运动中扮演的角色多元,性质复杂,不仅影响着华侨华人移民运动的发展,亦为华人信徒提供了一个普世性的社会流动网络,使基督宗教成为海外华人宗教的重要组成部分. 相似文献
75.
77.
Bi Zhu Chuansheng Chen Elizabeth F. Loftus Qinghua He Chunhui Chen Xuemei Lei Chongde Lin Qi Dong 《Applied cognitive psychology》2012,26(2):301-307
Do false memories last? And do they last as long as true ones? This study investigated whether experimentally created false memories would persist for an extended period (one and a half years). A large number of subjects (N = 342) participated in a standard three‐stage misinformation procedure (saw the event slides, read the narrations with misinformation, and then took the memory tests). The initial tests showed that misinformation led to a significant amount of false memory. One and a half years later, the participants were tested again. About half of the misinformation false memory persisted, which was the same rate as for true memory. These results strongly suggest that brief exposure to misinformation can lead to long‐term false memory and that the strength of memory trace was similar for true and false memories. Copyright © 2011 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. 相似文献
78.
It is established that stress impairs spatial learning and memory via the hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal axis response. Dopamine D1 receptors were also shown to be responsible for a stress-induced deficit of working memory. However, whether stress affects the subsequent emotional learning and memory is not elucidated yet. Here, we employed the well-established one-trial step-through task to study the effect of an acute psychological stress (induced by tail hanging for 5, 10, or 20?min) on emotional learning and memory, and the possible mechanisms as well. We demonstrated that tail hanging induced an obvious stress response. Either an acute tail-hanging stress or a single dose of intraperitoneally injected dopamine D1 receptor antagonist (SCH23390) significantly decreased the step-through latency in the one-trial step-through task. However, SCH23390 prevented the acute tail-hanging stress-induced decrease in the step-through latency. In addition, the effects of tail-hanging stress and/or SCH23390 on the changes in step-through latency were not through non-memory factors such as nociceptive perception and motor function. Our data indicate that the hyperactivation of dopamine D1 receptors mediated the stress-induced deficit of emotional learning and memory. This study may have clinical significance given that psychological stress is considered to play a role in susceptibility to some mental diseases such as depression and post-traumatic stress disorder. 相似文献
79.
Many studies have suggested that emotional stimuli orient and engage attention. There is also evidence that animate stimuli, such as those from humans and animals, cause attentional bias. However, categorical and emotional factors are usually mixed, and it is unclear to what extent human context influences attentional allocation. To address this issue, we tracked participants' eye movements while they viewed pictures with animals and inanimate images (i.e., category) as focal objects. These pictures had either negative or neutral emotional valence, and either human body parts or nonhuman parts were near the focal objects (i.e., context). The picture's valence, arousal, position, size, and most of the low-level visual features were matched across categories. The results showed that nonhuman animals were more likely to be attended to and to be attended to for longer times than inanimate objects. The same pattern held for the human contexts (vs. nonhuman contexts). The effects of emotional valence, category, and context interacted. Specifically, in images with a negative valence, focal animals and objects with human context had comparable numbers of gaze fixations and gaze duration. These results highlighted the attentional bias to animate parts of a picture and clarified that the effects of category, valence, and picture context interacted to influence attentional allocation. 相似文献
80.