全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1245篇 |
免费 | 20篇 |
专业分类
1265篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 9篇 |
2023年 | 7篇 |
2021年 | 11篇 |
2020年 | 23篇 |
2019年 | 41篇 |
2018年 | 52篇 |
2017年 | 49篇 |
2016年 | 56篇 |
2015年 | 35篇 |
2014年 | 44篇 |
2013年 | 159篇 |
2012年 | 63篇 |
2011年 | 82篇 |
2010年 | 45篇 |
2009年 | 50篇 |
2008年 | 52篇 |
2007年 | 66篇 |
2006年 | 51篇 |
2005年 | 39篇 |
2004年 | 38篇 |
2003年 | 38篇 |
2002年 | 26篇 |
2001年 | 20篇 |
2000年 | 12篇 |
1999年 | 10篇 |
1998年 | 14篇 |
1996年 | 8篇 |
1995年 | 5篇 |
1994年 | 5篇 |
1992年 | 7篇 |
1989年 | 5篇 |
1988年 | 5篇 |
1987年 | 4篇 |
1986年 | 5篇 |
1985年 | 6篇 |
1984年 | 6篇 |
1983年 | 5篇 |
1981年 | 5篇 |
1980年 | 4篇 |
1979年 | 7篇 |
1978年 | 4篇 |
1977年 | 5篇 |
1976年 | 8篇 |
1974年 | 5篇 |
1973年 | 6篇 |
1972年 | 4篇 |
1971年 | 7篇 |
1970年 | 5篇 |
1968年 | 8篇 |
1967年 | 7篇 |
排序方式: 共有1265条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
181.
The present study was designed to examine the processes by which grammatical gender is assigned during word production. French words varied in strength of sublexical cues, based on whether the word ending was typical for one gender rather than neutral about gender, and lexical cues, derived from the associated definite article being uninformative about gender for words beginning with a vowel, but informative for words beginning with a consonant. In Experiment 1, when native French speakers classified the gender of mentally evoked names of pictures, no effects of these cues were obtained. Experiment 2 used an improved methodology, with participants classifying the gender of words translated from English. English-speaking learners of French were influenced strongly by lexical and sublexical cues, while French speakers showed a weaker influence. However, for both speaker groups, words whose gender was classified slowly during recognition were also classified slowly during production, and error rates were similarly correlated across tasks. The conclusion was that gender is not equally available for all words once the associated “lemma” is accessed. Current models of language production may have to incorporate mechanisms allowing differential speed of access to gender information depending on a word’s formal properties. 相似文献
182.
Linden J. Ball Jaswinder. Shoker Jeremy N. V. Miles 《British journal of psychology (London, England : 1953)》2010,101(4):655-678
Previous studies examining environmental context‐dependent memory (ECDM) effects using indirect measures of memory have produced inconsistent findings. We report three experiments that examined ECDM in an indirect memory paradigm (word‐fragment completion) using ambient odours as environmental contexts. Expt 1 manipulated the odour present at learning and testing (rosemary or lemon) to produce reinstated‐context or switched‐context conditions. Reinstating rosemary led to a striking ECDM effect, indicating that indirect memory testing can be sensitive to ECDM manipulations. Odour ratings also indicated that rosemary induced a more unpleasant mood in participants than lemon. Expt 2 assessed the influence on indirect retrieval of odour‐based mood induction as well as odour distinctiveness, and indicated that rosemary's capacity to promote ECDM effects appears to arise from an additive combination of its unpleasantness‐inducing properties and its distinctiveness. Expt 3 partially supported these proposals. Overall, our findings indicate that some odours are capable of producing ECDM effects using indirect testing procedures. Moreover, it appears that it is the inherent proprieties of odours on dimensions such as unpleasantness and distinctiveness that mediate the emergence of ECDM effects, thereby explaining the particular potency of rosemary's mnemonic influence when it is reinstated. 相似文献
183.
Jeremy Shearmur 《Sophia》2010,49(4):475-488
This paper addresses the intellectual motivation of some of those involved in the intelligent design movement. It identifies
their concerns with the critique of the claim that Darwinism offers an adequate explanation of prima facie teleological features
in biology, a critique of naturalism, and the concern on the part of some of these authors including Dembski, with the revival
of 'Old Princeton' apologetics. It is argued that their work is interesting and is in principle intellectually legitimate.
It is also suggested, however, that it needs to be appraised qua 'research programme' (after the fashion of the early work
of Lakatos), and that, seen in that light, what needs to be accomplished might seem daunting. 相似文献
184.
Jeremy Gwiazda 《Sophia》2010,49(1):141-143
In this reply to Jesse Steinberg’s ‘God and the possibility of random creation’, I suggest a procedure whereby a being such
as God could randomly select a number from an infinite set. 相似文献
185.
Jeremy Gwiazda 《Philosophia》2010,38(2):357-363
Richard Swinburne’s argument in The Existence of God discusses many probabilities, ultimately concluding that God probably
exists. Swinburne gives exact values to almost none of these probabilities. I attempted to assign values to the probabilities
that met that weak condition that they could be correct. In this paper, I first present a brief outline of Swinburne’s argument
in The Existence of God. I then present the problems I encountered in Swinburne’s argument, specifically problems that interfered
with my attempt to arrive at values for the probabilities discussed by Swinburne. Finally, I suggest that Swinburne’s argument
would be more persuasive if he assigned exact values to his probabilities. 相似文献
186.
Dave Holmes Amélie M Perron Marc Savoie 《Philosophy, ethics, and humanities in medicine : PEHM》2006,1(1):12-6
This article outlines the struggle between the power of the health care professional and the rights of the individual to choose
freely a modality of treatment. Nurses are instrumental in assisting patients in making the best decision for a therapy they
will have to assume for the rest of their lives. In guiding patients' decision, nurses must take into account these unavoidable
contingencies: changes in lifestyle, nutritional restrictions, level of acceptance, compliance issues, ease of training and
availability of support/facilities. Ensuring that the patient makes an informed decision is therefore an ongoing challenge
for nurses as they are taking part in a delicate balancing act between not directly influencing the patient's decision while
making sure the patient is accurately informed. 相似文献
187.
Chad E. Morrow James A. Stephenson AnnaBelle O. Bryan Jeremy Haskell Mark Staal 《Military psychology》2013,25(6):568-576
Few studies have examined rates of mental health problems among special duty military personnel, who often have frequent deployments and high exposure to operational stressors and trauma. The current study examined the severity and rates of positive screening for posttraumatic stress, depression, and insomnia among 194 U.S. Air Force pararescuemen (PJs) in the active duty (AD) and National Guard/Reserve (NG/R) components. Overall estimated rates were 11.6% for probable posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), 1.6% for probable depression, and 16.1% for clinical insomnia. PJs in the NG/R reported significantly more severe posttraumatic stress symptoms (F(1, 162) = 10.031, p = .002, partial η2 = .058) and were approximately twice as likely to screen positive for probable PTSD (8.5% vs. 19.1%; χ2[1] = 3.679, p = .055). No differences in the rate or severity of depression or insomnia symptoms were found. Rates of positive screens are comparable to or lower than previously published rates among military personnel. 相似文献
188.
189.
Recent theories of attention have proposed that selection history is a separate, dissociable source of information that influences attention. The current study sought to investigate the simultaneous involvement of selection history and working-memory on attention during visual search. Experiments 1 and 2 used target feature probability to manipulate selection history and found significant effects of both working-memory and selection history, although working-memory dominated selection history when they cued different locations. Experiment 3 eliminated the contribution of voluntary refreshing of working-memory and replicated the main effects, although selection history became dominant. Using the same methodology, but with reduced probability cue validity, both effects were present in Experiment 4 and did not significantly differ in their contribution to attention. Effects of selection history and working-memory never interacted. These results suggest that selection history and working-memory are separate influences on attention and have little impact on each other. Theoretical implications for models of attention are discussed. 相似文献
190.
Jeremy Dauber 《Journal of Modern Jewish Studies》2013,12(1):104-109