全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1638篇 |
免费 | 72篇 |
专业分类
1710篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 8篇 |
2023年 | 24篇 |
2022年 | 34篇 |
2021年 | 55篇 |
2020年 | 47篇 |
2019年 | 53篇 |
2018年 | 104篇 |
2017年 | 80篇 |
2016年 | 73篇 |
2015年 | 51篇 |
2014年 | 59篇 |
2013年 | 203篇 |
2012年 | 126篇 |
2011年 | 124篇 |
2010年 | 94篇 |
2009年 | 38篇 |
2008年 | 105篇 |
2007年 | 73篇 |
2006年 | 70篇 |
2005年 | 39篇 |
2004年 | 37篇 |
2003年 | 26篇 |
2002年 | 25篇 |
2001年 | 11篇 |
2000年 | 18篇 |
1999年 | 19篇 |
1998年 | 7篇 |
1995年 | 4篇 |
1994年 | 5篇 |
1993年 | 5篇 |
1992年 | 7篇 |
1989年 | 4篇 |
1987年 | 6篇 |
1984年 | 2篇 |
1982年 | 2篇 |
1981年 | 2篇 |
1976年 | 2篇 |
1975年 | 2篇 |
1974年 | 3篇 |
1973年 | 7篇 |
1972年 | 3篇 |
1971年 | 8篇 |
1969年 | 3篇 |
1968年 | 10篇 |
1966年 | 4篇 |
1965年 | 4篇 |
1963年 | 3篇 |
1962年 | 2篇 |
1961年 | 2篇 |
1960年 | 2篇 |
排序方式: 共有1710条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
61.
62.
63.
Salvador Algarabel Manuel Fuentes Joaquín Escudero Alfonso Pitarque Vicente Peset José-Francisco Mazón 《Neuropsychology, development, and cognition. Section B, Aging, neuropsychology and cognition》2013,20(5):608-619
ABSTRACT There is no agreement on the pattern of recognition memory deficits characteristic of patients diagnosed with mild cognitive impairment (MCI). Whereas lower performance in recollection is the hallmark of MCI, there is a strong controversy about possible deficits in familiarity estimates when using recognition memory tasks. The aim of this research is to shed light on the pattern of responding in recollection and familiarity in MCI. Five groups of participants were tested. The main participant samples were those formed by two MCI groups differing in age and an Alzheimer's disease group (AD), which were compared with two control groups. Whereas one of the control groups served to assess the performance of the MCI and AD people, the other one, composed of young healthy participants, served the purpose of evaluating the adequacy of the experimental tasks used in the evaluation of the different components of recognition memory. We used an associative recognition task as a direct index of recollection and a choice task on a pair of stimuli, one of which was perceptually similar to those studied in the associative recognition phase, as an index of familiarity. Our results indicate that recollection decreases with age and neurological status, and familiarity remains stable in the elderly control sample but it is deficient in MCI. This research shows that a unique encoding situation generated deficits in recollective and familiarity mechanisms in mild cognitive impaired individuals, providing evidence for the existence of deficits in both retrieval processes in recognition memory in a MCI stage. 相似文献
64.
The ability to discriminate between larger and smaller quantities has been demonstrated in several mammalian and avian species
suggesting the possibility of evolutionary conservation of this characteristic. Preference for the larger of two groups has
also been shown in fish species, although this ability has rarely been systematically studied in lower order vertebrates,
and thus the mechanisms of such ability are not understood. Here, we exploit the tendency of angelfish to seek protection
in an unfamiliar environment by joining a group of conspecifics, a behaviour called shoaling. Test fish were given a simultaneous
choice between shoals varying both in terms of numerical ratios and absolute numbers of fish. Our results provide evidence
for quantity discrimination in angelfish. In general, experimental subjects chose the larger of two shoals. Furthermore, in
agreement with Weber’s law, which holds that discrimination between two quantities depends on their ratio, the discrimination
between shoals of different quantities of fish was more difficult when the shoal sizes became more similar. The limit of discrimination
ratio was found to be below 2:1. Briefly, angelfish are able to discriminate between different quantities of conspecifics
subject to a ratio limit, a finding that implies a fitness component in this behaviour similar to what has been demonstrated
in higher order vertebrates. 相似文献
65.
Koronczai B Urbán R Kökönyei G Paksi B Papp K Kun B Arnold P Kállai J Demetrovics Z 《Cyberpsychology, behavior and social networking》2011,14(11):657-664
As the Internet became widely used, problems associated with its excessive use became increasingly apparent. Although for the assessment of these problems several models and related questionnaires have been elaborated, there has been little effort made to confirm them. The aim of the present study was to test the three-factor model of the previously created Problematic Internet Use Questionnaire (PIUQ) by data collection methods formerly not applied (off-line group and face-to-face settings), on the one hand, and by testing on different age groups (adolescent and adult representative samples), on the other hand. Data were collected from 438 high-school students (44.5 percent boys; mean age: 16.0 years; standard deviation=0.7 years) and also from 963 adults (49.9 percent males; mean age: 33.6 years; standard deviation=11.8 years). We applied confirmatory factor analysis to confirm the measurement model of problematic Internet use. The results of the analyses carried out inevitably support the original three-factor model over the possible one-factor solution. Using latent profile analysis, we identified 11 percent of adults and 18 percent of adolescent users characterized by problematic use. Based on exploratory factor analysis, we also suggest a short form of the PIUQ consisting of nine items. Both the original 18-item version of PIUQ and its short 9-item form have satisfactory reliability and validity characteristics, and thus, they are suitable for the assessment of problematic Internet use in future studies. 相似文献
66.
Calderón-Garcidueñas L Engle R Mora-Tiscareño A Styner M Gómez-Garza G Zhu H Jewells V Torres-Jardón R Romero L Monroy-Acosta ME Bryant C González-González LO Medina-Cortina H D'Angiulli A 《Brain and cognition》2011,77(3):345-355
Exposure to severe air pollution produces neuroinflammation and structural brain alterations in children. We tested whether patterns of brain growth, cognitive deficits and white matter hyperintensities (WMH) are associated with exposures to severe air pollution. Baseline and 1 year follow-up measurements of global and regional brain MRI volumes, cognitive abilities (Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Revised, WISC-R), and serum inflammatory mediators were collected in 20 Mexico City (MC) children (10 with white matter hyperintensities, WMH+, and 10 without, WMH−) and 10 matched controls (CTL) from a low polluted city. There were significant differences in white matter volumes between CTL and MC children - both WMH+ and WMH− - in right parietal and bilateral temporal areas. Both WMH− and WMH+ MC children showed progressive deficits, compared to CTL children, on the WISC-R Vocabulary and Digit Span subtests. The cognitive deficits in highly exposed children match the localization of the volumetric differences detected over the 1 year follow-up, since the deficits observed are consistent with impairment of parietal and temporal lobe functions. Regardless of the presence of prefrontal WMH, Mexico City children performed more poorly across a variety of cognitive tests, compared to CTL children, thus WMH+ is likely only partially identifying underlying white matter pathology. Together these findings reveal that exposure to air pollution may perturb the trajectory of cerebral development and result in cognitive deficits during childhood. 相似文献
67.
Megías JL Romero-Sánchez M Durán M Moya M Bohner G 《The Spanish journal of psychology》2011,14(2):912-925
Two studies were conducted with college students to validate the Spanish-language version of the "Acceptance of Modern Myths about Sexual Aggression" scale (AMMSA) (Gerger, Kley, Bohner & Siebler, 2007). This scale assesses modern myths about sexual aggression in a subtle way. In Study 1, 305 students completed the Spanish AMMSA and other scales with related content. In Study 2, 263 participants completed the Spanish AMMSA and answered questions about a hypothetical sexual assault perpetrated by a young man against a female acquaintance. The Spanish AMMSA showed high internal consistency and adequate evidence of validity in both studies. Compared to traditional scales of rape myth acceptance, mean scores on the AMMSA were higher and their distributions more closely approximated normality. These findings suggest that the Spanish version of the AMMSA scale is a useful instrument to study the social perception of sexual aggression. 相似文献
68.
Gómez-Lázaro E Arregi A Beitia G Vegas O Azpiroz A Garmendia L 《Stress (Amsterdam, Netherlands)》2011,14(5):537-548
This study aimed to analyze different behavioral profiles in response to chronic social defeat using the sensorial contact model. We hypothesized that a passive profile, unlike an active one, would be associated with behavioral and physiological characteristics related to depression. Six-week-old OF1 male mice were subjected to defeat for 21 consecutive days. A combination of cluster and discriminant analyses of the behavior exhibited during confrontation on Day 21 established two behavioral profiles: active (n?=?22) and passive (n?=?34). Passive mice, with a high level of immobility and low non-social exploration, had higher plasma corticosterone concentrations than active mice after 21 days of defeat. Three days after the last defeat, passive mice had lower corticosterone levels than manipulated-control mice (n?=?11). Higher levels of interleukin-6 and tumor necrosis factor-α (TNF-α) in the spleen and lower hippocampal brain-derived neurotrophic factor levels were observed in passive mice in comparison with those in active mice and the manipulated controls. The only differences observed in active mice in relation to the manipulated control were higher plasma corticosterone (Day 21) and TNF-α levels. The results show that different behavioral profiles in response to chronic defeat are associated with different physiological profiles, and that the passive profile presents physiological characteristics previously associated with depression. 相似文献
69.
This paper describes the development of a self-report scale for adolescents to assess positive youth development values. It presents a substantial number of psychometric results performed on a sample of 2,400 adolescents (1,068 boys and 1,332 girls) from 12 to 17 years ( M = 14.73, SD = 1.25), who were studying secondary education in Western Andalusia. The results provide evidence of the psychometric quality of items, cross-validity of a structure of eight first-order factors and three second-order factors, obtained through exploratory and confirmatory factor analysis, and adequate reliability. This study concludes that the scale presents adequate evidence of validity and reliability that allows assessment, especially in school contexts, of a broad set of values of particular relevance to positive youth development. 相似文献
70.
Daniel Ellsberg presented in Ellsberg (The Quarterly Journal of Economics 75:643–669, 1961) various examples questioning the
thesis that decision making under uncertainty can be reduced to decision making under risk. These examples constitute one
of the main challenges to the received view on the foundations of decision theory offered by Leonard Savage in Savage (1972).
Craig Fox and Amos Tversky have, nevertheless, offered an indirect defense of Savage. They provided in Fox and Tversky (1995)
an explanation of Ellsberg’s two-color problem in terms of a psychological effect: ambiguity aversion. The ‘comparative ignorance’ hypothesis articulates how this effect works and explains why it is important to an understanding
of the typical pattern of responses associated with Ellsberg’s two-color problem. In the first part of this article we challenge
Fox and Tversky’s explanation. We present first an experiment that extends Ellsberg’s two-color problem where certain predictions
of the comparative ignorance hypothesis are not confirmed. In addition the hypothesis seems unable to explain how the subjects
resolve trade-offs between security and expected pay-off when vagueness is present. Ellsberg offered an explanation of the
typical behavior elicited by his examples in terms of these trade-offs and in section three we offer a model of Ellsberg’s
trade-offs. The model takes seriously the role of imprecise probabilities in explaining Ellsberg’s phenomenon. The so-called
three-color problem was also considered in Fox and Tversky (1995). We argue that Fox and Tversky’s analysis of this case breaks
a symmetry with their analysis of the two-color problem. We propose a unified treatment of both problems and we present a
experiment that confirms our hypothesis. 相似文献