全文获取类型
收费全文 | 903篇 |
免费 | 6篇 |
专业分类
909篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 6篇 |
2020年 | 14篇 |
2019年 | 16篇 |
2018年 | 29篇 |
2017年 | 15篇 |
2016年 | 24篇 |
2015年 | 6篇 |
2014年 | 11篇 |
2013年 | 110篇 |
2012年 | 31篇 |
2011年 | 23篇 |
2010年 | 21篇 |
2009年 | 30篇 |
2008年 | 28篇 |
2007年 | 47篇 |
2006年 | 30篇 |
2005年 | 27篇 |
2004年 | 22篇 |
2003年 | 31篇 |
2002年 | 26篇 |
2001年 | 16篇 |
2000年 | 30篇 |
1999年 | 11篇 |
1998年 | 16篇 |
1997年 | 7篇 |
1996年 | 12篇 |
1994年 | 9篇 |
1993年 | 6篇 |
1992年 | 9篇 |
1990年 | 13篇 |
1989年 | 9篇 |
1988年 | 9篇 |
1987年 | 10篇 |
1986年 | 11篇 |
1985年 | 12篇 |
1984年 | 13篇 |
1983年 | 7篇 |
1982年 | 7篇 |
1981年 | 6篇 |
1978年 | 6篇 |
1976年 | 7篇 |
1975年 | 6篇 |
1973年 | 11篇 |
1970年 | 7篇 |
1966年 | 5篇 |
1959年 | 7篇 |
1957年 | 6篇 |
1953年 | 5篇 |
1951年 | 8篇 |
1950年 | 6篇 |
排序方式: 共有909条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
751.
Jamal K. Mansour Jennifer L. Beaudry Mai-Tram Nguyen Roy Groncki 《Applied cognitive psychology》2024,38(6):e70001
Identification accuracy can be predicted from eyewitnesses' self-reported decision processes but the evidence of their ability to improve prediction when confidence and response time are included is mixed and minimal. Typically, decision processes are measured via one or five self-report questions; we explored whether a more nuanced questionnaire could improve prediction. Participants viewed a mock-crime video, made a target-present or -absent lineup decision, and completed 17 decision process items. An exploratory factor analysis on choosers' (n = 391) responses revealed three correlated factors, broadly reflecting automatic response, relative judgment, and absolute judgment. The three-factor solution had good internal reliability (McDonald's ωs = 0.93, 0.89, and 0.74, respectively). Scores produced from the questions loading on the automatic response and relative judgment factors improved predictions of accuracy compared to using confidence and response time alone. Self-reported decision processes may be an easy-to-administer and useful reflector of identification accuracy. 相似文献
752.
Roy R. Spina Li‐Jun Ji Michael Ross Ye Li Zhiyong Zhang 《Asian Journal of Social Psychology》2010,13(3):153-162
Four studies were conducted to investigate cultural differences in predicting and understanding regression toward the mean. We demonstrated, with tasks in such domains as athletic competition, health and weather, that Chinese are more likely than Canadians to make predictions that are consistent with regression toward the mean. In addition, Chinese are more likely than Canadians to choose a regression‐consistent explanation to account for regression toward the mean. The findings are consistent with cultural differences in lay theories about how people, objects and events develop over time. 相似文献
753.
Lanny Fields Robert Travis Deborah Roy Eytan Yadlovker Liliane de Aguiar‐Rocha Peter Sturmey 《Journal of applied behavior analysis》2009,42(3):575-593
Many students struggle with statistical concepts such as interaction. In an experimental group, participants took a paper‐and‐pencil test and then were given training to establish equivalent classes containing four different statistical interactions. All participants formed the equivalence classes and showed maintenance when probes contained novel negative exemplars. Thereafter, participants took a second paper‐and‐pencil test. Participants in the control group received two versions of the paper‐and‐pencil test without equivalence‐based instruction. All participants in the experimental group showed increased paper‐and‐pencil test scores after forming the interaction‐indicative equivalence classes. Class‐indicative responding also generalized to novel exemplars and the novel question format used in the paper‐and‐pencil test. Test scores did not change with repetition for control group participants. Implications for behavioral diagnostics and teaching technology are discussed. 相似文献
754.
Roy F. Baumeister 《Motivation and emotion》2016,40(1):1-10
Motivation theories have tended to focus on specific motivations, leaving open the intellectually and scientifically challenging problem of how to construct a general theory of motivation. The requirements for such a theory are presented here. The primacy of motivation emphasizes that cognition, emotion, agency, and other psychological processes exist to serve motivation. Both state (impulses) and trait (basic drives) forms of motivation must be explained, and their relationship must be illuminated. Not all motivations are the same, and indeed it is necessary to explain how motivation evolved from the simple desires of simple animals into the complex, multifaceted forms of human motivation. Motivation responds to the local environment but may also adapt to it, such as when desires increase after satiation or diminish when satisfaction is chronically unavailable. Addiction may be a special case of motivation—but perhaps it is much less special or different than prevailing cultural stereotypes suggest. The relationship between liking and wanting, and the self-regulatory management of motivational conflict, also require explanation by an integrative theory. 相似文献
755.
Anjali Gera Roy 《Sikh Formations》2016,12(1):67-86
The Komagata Maru episode, which became a test case for White Canada and Asian Exclusion polices, epitomizes the process of ‘the making and unmaking of strangers’ under British imperialism (Bauman 1997). While the passengers on board the ship chartered by Gurdit Singh, a descendant of one of the Sikhs who had arrived in the British Malaya as part of Captain Speedy’s force in 1872, viewed themselves as British subjects who were free to move within the borders of the British Empire that included the Dominion of Canada, the Immigration Act of 1910 posited the immigrant, as Audrey Macklin convincingly argues, as the other of the Canadian subject (2011). The Komagata Maru episode strongly underlines grave inconsistencies in the definition of the stranger in different parts of the British Empire. In movements triggered by imperial policies and agendas such as those to Shanghai, the British Malaya or East Africa, the Sikh was regarded as a favourable stranger. In movements initiated by himself, the Sikh was resignified as a hostile stranger and his movements were closely regulated. This paper will closely examine newspaper reports, letters, telegrams and witness statements of the Japanese crew and British officials in the Komagata Maru Inquiry Committee Report to focus on the resignification of the Sikhs from favourable to hostile strangers under the British Empire through their being labelled as aliens, undesirable persons and ‘dangerous seditionists’ prejudicial to the safety and tranquility of the British state. 相似文献
756.
Proposed somatic cell gene-therapies (especially those involving in utero therapies) may involve a small risk of germ-line modifications; this risk has engendered serious concern, and arguments have been made that such therapies ought not be pursued if such risks exists. We argue here that while pursuing deliberate germ-line modifications in humans would be inappropriate given the current state of the art, the risk of accidental germ-line modifications from most currently proposed in utero gene therapy is no different in kind or degree from other risks regularly taken in medical procedures. Given the possible benefits of such therapies, we argue that the risk of accidental germ-line modifications is well worth taking in these cases. 相似文献
757.
Philosophical Studies - 相似文献
758.
Moore's Problem with Iterated Belief 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Roy Sorenson 《The Philosophical quarterly》2000,50(198):28-43
G.E. Moore's problem is to explain what is odd about sentences such as 'It is raining but I do not believe it'. This sentence is unbelievable whether read commissively as ' p & B ˜/sup> p ', or omissively as ' p & ˜/sup> B p '. However, this symmetry is broken when the belief operator is iterated many times. Whereas the commissive ' p & BBBBBBBBBB˜ p ' preserves the oddity Moore first noted, ' p & ˜ BBBBBBBBBB p ' is believable and assertable. The usual explanations of what is odd about Moore's sentences use recursive rules. Hence they uniformly extend the prediction of oddity when the belief operator is iterated. My project is to show how the asymmetry of the iterated version can be respected. 相似文献
759.
760.
Roy F. Ellen 《Journal of the history of the behavioral sciences》1976,12(4):303-324
Although there have been studies of both Dutch colonial policy in the Indies and the development of anthropology in the Netherlands, there has been no systematic examination of the historical relations between them. This paper attempts this for a period of 160 years from the collapse of the Dutch East India Company to the birth of an independent Indonesian state. During this time, the need of successive governments for information on subject peoples was matched by the requirements of scholars for suitable conditions and locations for their work. As Dutch anthropology emerged in the nineteenth century and developed in the twentieth, it was closely related to the prevailing political climate—state capitalism, liberal, and ethical policies. The analysis shows how there is a “fit” between these and certain dominant anthropological styles and interests, principally in the form of empiricism, customary law studies, “Leiden” structuralism, and functionalism. 相似文献