首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   529篇
  免费   18篇
  2020年   6篇
  2019年   9篇
  2018年   8篇
  2017年   12篇
  2016年   15篇
  2015年   9篇
  2014年   8篇
  2013年   53篇
  2012年   17篇
  2011年   20篇
  2010年   8篇
  2009年   7篇
  2008年   18篇
  2007年   18篇
  2006年   20篇
  2005年   14篇
  2004年   18篇
  2003年   12篇
  2002年   18篇
  2001年   20篇
  2000年   13篇
  1999年   12篇
  1998年   9篇
  1997年   5篇
  1996年   8篇
  1995年   10篇
  1992年   12篇
  1990年   10篇
  1989年   13篇
  1988年   7篇
  1987年   6篇
  1986年   4篇
  1985年   10篇
  1983年   5篇
  1981年   4篇
  1980年   4篇
  1979年   6篇
  1978年   6篇
  1977年   7篇
  1976年   4篇
  1975年   7篇
  1974年   8篇
  1973年   6篇
  1971年   4篇
  1970年   5篇
  1969年   5篇
  1968年   5篇
  1967年   4篇
  1966年   9篇
  1965年   3篇
排序方式: 共有547条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
351.
This paper examines the proposition that the mobilization of energy and consequent magnitude of valence of a potential outcome (e.g., goal) is a function of what the individual perceives can and must be done in order to attain or avoid the outcome. An outcome that is difficult to attain or avoid requires a relatively high level of energization and will be relatively attractive, if positive, or unpleasant, if negative. Outcomes that are easy or impossible to attain or avoid require little or no energization and will be relatively low in attractiveness, if positive, or low in unpleasantness, if negative. This formulation was supported by four experiments that demonstrated (a) attractiveness of a goal is a nonmonotonic function of perceived difficulty of attaining it; (b) unpleasantness of a potential negative outcome is a nonmonotonic function of perceived difficulty of avoiding it; (c) the nonmonotonic effect of perceived difficulty on goal attractiveness disappears once instrumental behavior has been completed; and (d) the nonmonotonic effect of perceived difficulty on unpleasantness of a potential negative outcome occurs in immediate but not distant anticipation of initiating instrumental behavior. Alternative explanations, theoretical problems, and implications are discussed.  相似文献   
352.
This paper describes a microprocessor control system and solid state interface for controlling the apparatus in combined behavioral-electrophysiological studies of conditioning. The computer program, which is designed to control classical conditioning of the rabbit’s nictitating membrane response, provides the flexibility to control all conditioning parameters (e.g., interstimulus and intertrial intervals, trial type, and sequence of trial types) with only minor modifications. The system is free from artifacts that can distort electrophysiological recordings and can easily be modified to accommodate other behavioral paradigms in which electrophysiological responses are recorded.  相似文献   
353.
Peer and teacher interactions with five "disruptive" children were studied in an elementary school classroom. The intent of the study was to analyze experimentally peer reinforcement control of the disruptive children's problem behaviors. Social attention provided by all peers was found to be directed exclusively to the problem behaviors during baseline. Following baseline, several manipulations of selected peer social attention demonstrated the reinforcement function of this stimulus class.  相似文献   
354.
355.
356.
A mechanism for the disinhibition of displacement activities is proposed, by which a factor arising from non-reward causes attention to be switched to irrelevant aspects of the stimulus situation. In addition to evidence already extant, three lines of evidence are presented for the case of displacement pecking in the Barbary dove: (1) partially rewarded doves learn more about incidental aspects of a discrimination situation than consistently rewarded doves; (2) partially rewarded doves respond more readily to irrelevant stimuli than consistently rewarded doves; (3) doves which take longest to extinguish a simple approach response, also spend most time displacement pecking.  相似文献   
357.
Two models of group behavior in the solution of eureka-type problems   总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2  
A study by Shaw (7) some twenty years ago is frequently cited by social scientists to support the generalization that groups are superior to individuals in problem-solving. Shaw suggests that personal interaction within the group is responsible for the superior performance of groups. This article re-examines her data in the light of two models which propose that the difference in quality of solution between group and individual performance is solely a matter of ability. It is shown that Shaw's data may be considered to have been an outcome of behavior postulated by the models. Since Shaw's observations relate to a special population and to special kinds of problems, the proposed models may not be appropriate under differing experimental conditions. In fact, Lorgeet al. (4) have indicated that experimental demonstration of the superiority of groups over individuals in problem-solving depends not only on the kind of group but also on the kind of problem to be solved. In addition, the diversity of transfer of training for groups and for individuals is considered.Supported in part by the Office of Naval Research under Contract N6 onr 266 (21) and the Air Force Personnel and Training Research Center under Contract AF 18(600)-341.  相似文献   
358.
This study assessed the beliefs and cognitions of bus-train collision survivors, 7 years following the accident. The sample consisted of 389 young adults who were adolescents at the time of the disaster. The sample was composed of 4 groups who differed in their levels of exposure to traumatic stress. Results indicated that (a) exposure to the traumatic accident was implicated in challenging survivors' beliefs about the benevolence of the world. Justice and luck were also associated with posttraumatic psychiatric and functional impairment; (b) the level of exposure had a direct bearing on dependent variables; and (c) cognitive schemata were associated with psychiatric symptomatology and problems in functioning, reflecting the coexistence of diverse traumatic sequelae. Theoretical implications of these results are discussed.  相似文献   
359.
360.
The time needed to compare two symbols increases as the cognitive distance between them on the relevant dimension increases (symbolic distance effect). Furthermore, when subjects are told to choose either the larger or the smaller of two stimuli, the response time is shorter if the instruction is congruent with the overall size of the stimuli (semantic congruity effect). Three experiments were conducted to determine the locus of these effects in terms of a sequence of processing stages. The developmental aspects of these effects were also evaluated, as the subjects were from kindergarten, first grade, third grade, fifth grade, and college. By varying the visual quality of the stimulus in each experiment, it was determined that the distance effect resides in a comparison stage, whereas the congruity effect is an encoding phenomenon. Both distance and congruity effects were present at all grade levels, but they decreased in magnitude as grade increased. The results were interpreted relative to recent models of comparative judgments.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号