首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   251篇
  免费   7篇
  2020年   2篇
  2019年   10篇
  2018年   6篇
  2017年   4篇
  2016年   10篇
  2015年   3篇
  2014年   8篇
  2013年   27篇
  2012年   7篇
  2011年   18篇
  2010年   6篇
  2009年   14篇
  2008年   12篇
  2007年   14篇
  2006年   4篇
  2005年   8篇
  2004年   9篇
  2003年   5篇
  2002年   4篇
  2001年   4篇
  2000年   7篇
  1999年   4篇
  1998年   7篇
  1997年   2篇
  1996年   4篇
  1995年   3篇
  1994年   3篇
  1992年   3篇
  1991年   2篇
  1990年   3篇
  1985年   3篇
  1984年   2篇
  1983年   3篇
  1980年   1篇
  1979年   2篇
  1977年   2篇
  1976年   1篇
  1975年   1篇
  1973年   2篇
  1972年   3篇
  1971年   3篇
  1970年   2篇
  1969年   1篇
  1968年   3篇
  1966年   2篇
  1965年   2篇
  1953年   1篇
  1936年   1篇
  1931年   1篇
  1926年   1篇
排序方式: 共有258条查询结果,搜索用时 31 毫秒
111.
The study investigated whether dynamic information promotes newborns' ability to recognize a face. After being habituated to a face undergoing an ordered sequential head rigid motion (Exp. 1), 1- to 3-day-old newborns fully recognized the familiar face shown in the profile pose, as documented by a preference for the profile pose of a novel face. When familiarized to an ordered sequence of different viewpoint static images of a face (Exp. 2), newborns failed to recognize the profile pose of the familiar face, their visual exploration of the familiar and novel faces being at chance. Habituation to a random head rigid motion (Exp. 3) allowed only a partial recognition of face identity, as newborns preferred the profile pose of the familiar, rather than the novel, face. Rigid head motion is an important source of information for newborns' face recognition, probably aiding the derivation of a three-dimensional face structure.  相似文献   
112.
113.
114.
Depression     
Patients with myocardial infarction who suffer from a depression are at increased risk of dying of a subsequent heart attack or some other complication of coronary artery disease (CAD). A considerable body of research has provided evidence that a major depression, which is found in 16 to 23 percent of patients with CAD, but also subliminal depressive symptoms are independent risk factors for an unfavourable outcome of CAD. However, it is not yet clear whether depression is a causal risk factor having impact on the course of the heart disease or merely a prognostic marker that allows predicting the outcomes of interest, without any causal influence on them. Several pathways between depression and CAD have been discussed. These include behavioral mechanisms such as low compliance with both medical treatment and life style recommendations as well as neurobiological links. Much attention has been paid to the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical and sympathomedullary hyperactivity found in depression. Other possible links include diminished heart rate variability, stress-induced ischemia, platelet activation, and immunological dysregulation. To resolve the issue whether depression is a causal risk factor or only a prognostic marker, experimental studies are needed to evaluate interventions aimed at improving depression and test whether mortality is subsequently reduced. Such studies brought in the past mixed results. Whereas comprehensive intervention programs including risk factor management have produced a reduction in both coronary morbidity and mortality, a recent multicenter study providing either cognitive-behavioral therapy or usual care to depressed patients with CAD could not demonstrate a survival benefit among the participants of the intervention. Finally, results of evaluation studies regarding integrated disease management programs for patients with comorbid depression are presented.  相似文献   
115.
We review 50-odd years of research on Allport's (1954)'contact hypothesis', to assess progress, problems, and prospects. We chart the progress that has been made in understanding two distinct forms of contact: direct and indirect. We highlight the progress made in understanding the effects of each type of contact, as well as both moderating and mediating factors, and emphasize the multiple impacts of direct contact, especially. We then consider some of the main critiques of inter-group contact, focusing on empirical issues and whether contact impedes social change, and provide a research agenda for the coming years. We conclude that this body of work no longer merits the modest title of 'hypothesis', but fully deserves acknowledgement as an integrated and influential theory.  相似文献   
116.
In 2001, a federal appeals court upheld the job termination of a counselor who requested being excused from counseling a lesbian client on relationship issues because homosexuality conflicted with the counselor's religious beliefs (Bruff v. North Mississippi Health Services, Inc., 2001). This article provides the facts of the case and the legal reasoning of the court. The authors also explore the legal and ethical issues related to this case.  相似文献   
117.
Julia Hermann 《Ratio》2019,32(4):300-311
Assuming that there is moral progress, and assuming that the abolition of slavery is an example of it, how does moral progress occur? Is it mainly driven by specific individuals who have gained new moral insights, or by changes in the socio‐economic and epistemic conditions in which agents morally judge the norms and practices of their society, and act upon these judgements? In this paper, I argue that moral progress is a complex process in which changes at the level of belief and changes at the level of institutions and social practices are deeply intertwined, and that changes in the socio‐economic and epistemic conditions of moral agency constitute the main motor of moral progress. I develop my view of moral progress by way of grappling with Michelle Moody‐Adams’ prominent philosophical account of it. My view is less intellectualistic and individualistic than hers, does not presuppose meta‐ethical moral realism, and blurs her distinction between moral progress in beliefs and moral progress in social practices. I point out the limits of humans to progress morally, which are partly grounded in our evolutionary history, and argue that moral progress is always of a ‘local’ nature.  相似文献   
118.
Visual working memory (VWM) is a core construct in the cognitive (neuro‐)sciences, assumed to serve as a hub for information exchange and thus supporting a multitude of cognitive functions related to processing visual information. Here, we give an introduction into key terms and paradigms and an overview of ongoing debates in the field, to which the articles collected in this Special Issue on ‘Current Directions in Visual Working Memory Research’ contribute. Our aim is to extract, from this overview, some ‘emerging’ theoretical insights concerning questions such as the optimal way to examine VWM, which types of mental representations contribute to performance on VWM tasks, and how VWM keeps features from the same object together and apart from features of concurrently maintained objects (the binding problem).  相似文献   
119.
Visual working memory (VWM) is a central bottleneck in human information processing. Its capacity is most often measured in terms of how many individual‐item representations VWM can hold (k). In the standard task employed to estimate k, an array of highly discriminable colour patches is maintained and, after a short retention interval, compared to a test display (change detection). Recent research has shown that with more complex, structured displays, change‐detection performance is, in addition to individual‐item representations, supported by ensemble representations formed as a result of spatial subgroupings. Here, by asking participants to additionally localize the change, we reveal indication for an influence of ensemble representations even in the very simple, unstructured displays of the colour‐patch change‐detection task. Critically, pure‐item models from which standard formulae of k are derived do not consider ensemble representations and, therefore, potentially overestimate k. To gauge this overestimation, we develop an item‐plus‐ensemble model of change detection and change localization. Estimates of k from this new model are about 1 item (~30%) lower than the estimates from traditional pure‐item models, even if derived from the same data sets.  相似文献   
120.
Blanz V  O'Toole AJ  Vetter T  Wild HA 《Perception》2000,29(8):885-891
We created a 'face space' using a laser-scan representation of faces. In this space, a caricature can be made by moving a face away from the average face, along the line connecting the particular face to the average face. Here, we move the face along this line in the other direction, proceeding through the mean and 'out the other side'. This results in a face that is 'opposite', in a computational sense, to the original face. We morphed several faces into their anti-faces and sampled the morph trajectory in five discrete steps. We then collected similarity ratings from human participants for all possible pairs of morphed faces to determine how the distances in the 'physical face space' related to the distances in the 'psychological face space'. The data indicate that there is a perceptual discontinuity of face identity as the face crosses over to the 'other side of the mean'. We consider these results in the context of face-space models of human face processing.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号