首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   288篇
  免费   10篇
  2022年   2篇
  2021年   3篇
  2020年   3篇
  2019年   10篇
  2018年   10篇
  2017年   6篇
  2016年   10篇
  2015年   5篇
  2014年   9篇
  2013年   32篇
  2012年   11篇
  2011年   20篇
  2010年   9篇
  2009年   15篇
  2008年   15篇
  2007年   16篇
  2006年   5篇
  2005年   11篇
  2004年   10篇
  2003年   5篇
  2002年   4篇
  2001年   6篇
  2000年   8篇
  1999年   5篇
  1998年   5篇
  1997年   2篇
  1996年   3篇
  1995年   3篇
  1993年   2篇
  1992年   3篇
  1991年   2篇
  1990年   3篇
  1989年   2篇
  1985年   3篇
  1984年   2篇
  1983年   4篇
  1977年   3篇
  1975年   2篇
  1974年   3篇
  1973年   2篇
  1972年   2篇
  1971年   3篇
  1970年   3篇
  1968年   1篇
  1966年   2篇
  1965年   1篇
  1953年   1篇
  1936年   1篇
  1931年   1篇
  1926年   1篇
排序方式: 共有298条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
131.
132.
Previous studies found little or no transfer of adaptation from reactive saccades to arm pointing movements, which suggests that the two motor systems rely on distinct adaptive mechanisms. However, this conclusion is based on experiments about the adaptation of response amplitudes, which is known to follow somewhat different principles than the adaptation of response directions. In the present study, we therefore investigate whether adapting the direction of reactive saccades will transfer to arm movements. We also test transfer in the opposite direction, from the arm to the eyes. Participants executed aimed saccades or arm movements from a central starting point towards visual targets in the participants' frontal plane. Targets were presented in eight possible locations along a circle of 20 cm radius about the starting point; each remained for 200 ms in one position, and was then displaced along the circle by -15 degrees . Participants from group E adapted to these double-stepped targets while executing eye movements, and were then tested for transfer while executing arm movements. The reciprocal design was used in participants from group A. Adaptive change in group A was about 14 degrees , while in group E it was only about 7 degrees . Transfer of adaptation was substantial, and was more pronounced when using the arm (i.e., eye-to-arm transfer in group E) rather than the eyes (i.e., arm-to-eye transfer in group A). Strong aftereffects were yielded in both groups. This pattern of findings implies that the adaptive change observed in our study was mainly based on recalibration rather than on cognitive strategies (strong aftereffects), that eyes and arm had access to a common adaptive mechanism (substantial transfer), and that the arm had better access than the eyes (larger adaptation and transfer when using the arm). When considering this outcome along with the available literature, it appears that arm and eyes may rely sometimes on a common and sometimes on distinct adaptive mechanisms, depending on the adapted parameter and on the nature of the motor task.  相似文献   
133.
134.
135.
Eye and hand movements can adapt to a variety of sensorimotor discordances. Studies on adaptation of movement directions suggest that the oculomotor and the hand motor system access the same adaptive mechanism related to the polarity of a discordance, because concurrent adaptations to opposite directed discordances strongly interfere. The authors scrutinized whether participants adapt their hand and eye movements to opposite directions (clockwise/counterclockwise) when both motor systems are alternatingly exposed to opposite directed double steps, and whether such adaptation is influenced by the allocation of effector to adaptation direction. The results showed that hand and eye movements adapted to opposite directions, but adaptation was biased to the counterclockwise direction. Aftereffects emerged nearly unbiased and independently for both motor systems. The authors conclude that the oculomotor and the hand motor system use independent mechanisms when they adapt to opposite polarities, although they interact during adaptation or concurrent performance.  相似文献   
136.
Depression     
Patients with myocardial infarction who suffer from a depression are at increased risk of dying of a subsequent heart attack or some other complication of coronary artery disease (CAD). A considerable body of research has provided evidence that a major depression, which is found in 16 to 23 percent of patients with CAD, but also subliminal depressive symptoms are independent risk factors for an unfavourable outcome of CAD. However, it is not yet clear whether depression is a causal risk factor having impact on the course of the heart disease or merely a prognostic marker that allows predicting the outcomes of interest, without any causal influence on them. Several pathways between depression and CAD have been discussed. These include behavioral mechanisms such as low compliance with both medical treatment and life style recommendations as well as neurobiological links. Much attention has been paid to the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical and sympathomedullary hyperactivity found in depression. Other possible links include diminished heart rate variability, stress-induced ischemia, platelet activation, and immunological dysregulation. To resolve the issue whether depression is a causal risk factor or only a prognostic marker, experimental studies are needed to evaluate interventions aimed at improving depression and test whether mortality is subsequently reduced. Such studies brought in the past mixed results. Whereas comprehensive intervention programs including risk factor management have produced a reduction in both coronary morbidity and mortality, a recent multicenter study providing either cognitive-behavioral therapy or usual care to depressed patients with CAD could not demonstrate a survival benefit among the participants of the intervention. Finally, results of evaluation studies regarding integrated disease management programs for patients with comorbid depression are presented.  相似文献   
137.
We review 50-odd years of research on Allport's (1954)'contact hypothesis', to assess progress, problems, and prospects. We chart the progress that has been made in understanding two distinct forms of contact: direct and indirect. We highlight the progress made in understanding the effects of each type of contact, as well as both moderating and mediating factors, and emphasize the multiple impacts of direct contact, especially. We then consider some of the main critiques of inter-group contact, focusing on empirical issues and whether contact impedes social change, and provide a research agenda for the coming years. We conclude that this body of work no longer merits the modest title of 'hypothesis', but fully deserves acknowledgement as an integrated and influential theory.  相似文献   
138.
In 2001, a federal appeals court upheld the job termination of a counselor who requested being excused from counseling a lesbian client on relationship issues because homosexuality conflicted with the counselor's religious beliefs (Bruff v. North Mississippi Health Services, Inc., 2001). This article provides the facts of the case and the legal reasoning of the court. The authors also explore the legal and ethical issues related to this case.  相似文献   
139.
Julia Hermann 《Ratio》2019,32(4):300-311
Assuming that there is moral progress, and assuming that the abolition of slavery is an example of it, how does moral progress occur? Is it mainly driven by specific individuals who have gained new moral insights, or by changes in the socio‐economic and epistemic conditions in which agents morally judge the norms and practices of their society, and act upon these judgements? In this paper, I argue that moral progress is a complex process in which changes at the level of belief and changes at the level of institutions and social practices are deeply intertwined, and that changes in the socio‐economic and epistemic conditions of moral agency constitute the main motor of moral progress. I develop my view of moral progress by way of grappling with Michelle Moody‐Adams’ prominent philosophical account of it. My view is less intellectualistic and individualistic than hers, does not presuppose meta‐ethical moral realism, and blurs her distinction between moral progress in beliefs and moral progress in social practices. I point out the limits of humans to progress morally, which are partly grounded in our evolutionary history, and argue that moral progress is always of a ‘local’ nature.  相似文献   
140.
Visual working memory (VWM) is a core construct in the cognitive (neuro‐)sciences, assumed to serve as a hub for information exchange and thus supporting a multitude of cognitive functions related to processing visual information. Here, we give an introduction into key terms and paradigms and an overview of ongoing debates in the field, to which the articles collected in this Special Issue on ‘Current Directions in Visual Working Memory Research’ contribute. Our aim is to extract, from this overview, some ‘emerging’ theoretical insights concerning questions such as the optimal way to examine VWM, which types of mental representations contribute to performance on VWM tasks, and how VWM keeps features from the same object together and apart from features of concurrently maintained objects (the binding problem).  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号