首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   1694篇
  免费   118篇
  2023年   16篇
  2022年   15篇
  2021年   27篇
  2020年   53篇
  2019年   48篇
  2018年   72篇
  2017年   65篇
  2016年   86篇
  2015年   67篇
  2014年   56篇
  2013年   258篇
  2012年   84篇
  2011年   104篇
  2010年   65篇
  2009年   72篇
  2008年   106篇
  2007年   85篇
  2006年   55篇
  2005年   48篇
  2004年   38篇
  2003年   48篇
  2002年   44篇
  2001年   46篇
  2000年   33篇
  1999年   22篇
  1998年   16篇
  1997年   13篇
  1996年   16篇
  1995年   9篇
  1994年   5篇
  1993年   6篇
  1991年   8篇
  1990年   7篇
  1989年   4篇
  1985年   8篇
  1984年   5篇
  1983年   9篇
  1982年   8篇
  1980年   4篇
  1978年   4篇
  1977年   3篇
  1976年   3篇
  1975年   7篇
  1973年   6篇
  1972年   4篇
  1971年   5篇
  1970年   3篇
  1966年   3篇
  1938年   3篇
  1937年   3篇
排序方式: 共有1812条查询结果,搜索用时 203 毫秒
271.
272.
Emotional distress connected to a cancer disease may result in comorbid mental and especially depressive disorders. Although psychodynamic therapy is one of the most frequently applied forms of psychotherapy of depression in cancer patients there is a lack of empirical studies which meet the criteria of evidence-based psychotherapy research. This is especially true with regard to randomized controlled trials of psychodynamic psychotherapy. As far as we know no manual for psychodynamic treatment of cancer patients has yet been published. In this article a manual for a 20-session short-term psychodynamic treatment is presented. The treatment is based on Luborsky’s supportive-expressive (SE) therapy. The SE treatment manual of depressions in cancer patients presented here is an adaptation of the general SET manual as well as the SE manual for depression. In the first part the general principles of SE therapy are presented. In the second part a manual specifically adapted to the treatment of depressive cancer patients is described. The treatment manual is presently being used in a large-scale randomized controlled multi-centered study comparing short-term psychodynamic psychotherapy and treatment as usual.  相似文献   
273.
274.
Two- to 8-month-old infants interacted with their mother or a stranger in a prospective longitudinal gaze following study. Gaze following, as assessed by eye tracking, emerged between 2 and 4 months and stabilized between 6 and 8 months of age. Overall, infants followed the gaze of a stranger more than they followed the gaze of their mothers, demonstrating a stranger preference that emerged between 4 and 6 months of age. These findings do not support the notion that infants acquire gaze following through reinforcement learning. Instead, the findings are discussed with respect to the social cognitive framework, suggesting that young infants are driven by social cognitive motives in their interactions with others.  相似文献   
275.
Depressive St?rungen geh?ren bei Allgemeinkrankenhauspatienten zu den h?ufigsten psychischen St?rungen. Sie haben oft negative Auswirkungen auf die Prognose und die Behandlung k?rperlicher Erkrankungen. Selbst für den routinierten und erfahrenen Arzt stellt die antidepressive Pharmakotherapie depressiver Patienten mit somatischen Krankheiten oftmals eine besondere Herausforderung dar. In diesem Kontext gilt es in der Regel, unerwünschte Arzneimittelwirkungen bei Komedikation mit anderen Pharmaka – wie zum Beispiel den Wirkverlust eines zuvor in stabilen Erhaltungsdosen verabreichten Medikaments oder ein erh?htes Risiko für die Entwicklung von Arzneimittelnebenwirkungen – zu vermeiden.  相似文献   
276.
277.
We demonstrate qualitative dissociations of brightness processing in visuomotor priming and conscious vision. Speeded keypress responses to the brighter of two luminance targets were performed in the presence of preceding dark and bright primes (clearly visible and flanking the targets) whose apparent brightness values were enhanced or attenuated by a visual illusion. Response times to the targets were greatly affected by consistent versus inconsistent arrangements of the primes, relative to the targets (response priming). Priming effects could systematically contradict subjective brightness matches, such that one prime could appear brighter than the other but could prime as if it were darker. Systematic variation of the illusion showed that response-priming effects depended only on local flanker-background contrast, not on the subjective appearance of the flankers. Our findings suggest that speeded motor responses, as opposed to conscious perceptual judgments, access an early phase of lightness and brightness processing prior to full lightness constancy.  相似文献   
278.
Continuity of father-rated temperament in Finnish children (n = 115) aged from 6 months (the IBQ) to 5.5 years (the CBQ) was explored within the theoretical framework developed by Rothbart. Father-rated activity level, smiling and laughter and distress to limitations showed significant differential homotypic and heterotypic continuity, while soothability, duration of orienting and fear showed significant differential heterotypic continuity. On the level of latent superconstructs, father-rated positive and negative infant affectivity accounted for 5.1, 22.7 and 10.0% of the variance in childhood extraversion, effortful control and negative affectivity, respectively. Inter-parental comparisons (n = 109 family units) revealed that father-rated continuity was similar to mother-rated continuity. These findings give empirical credence to Rothbart's theory and to father-rated temperamental continuity from infancy onwards.  相似文献   
279.
The earliest electrophysiological correlate of visual awareness?   总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2  
To examine the neural correlates and timing of human visual awareness, we recorded event-related potentials (ERPs) in two experiments while the observers were detecting a grey dot that was presented near subjective threshold. ERPs were averaged for conscious detections of the stimulus (hits) and nondetections (misses) separately. Our results revealed that hits, as compared to misses, showed a negativity around 180-350 ms at occipital and posterior temporal sites. It was followed by a positive wave after 400-500 ms, peaking at parietal sites. These correlates were not affected by a manipulation of attention. The early negativity, called 'visual awareness negativity' (VAN), may be a general, primary electrophysiological correlate of visual awareness. The present data show that it can be observed in response to appearance of a stimulus in visual awareness and that it generalizes across different manipulations of stimulus visibility.  相似文献   
280.
Three experiments re-examined Baylis and Driver's (1993) strong evidence for object-based selection, that making relative apex location judgments is harder between two objects than within a single object, with object (figure-ground) segmentation determined solely by color-based perceptual set. Using variations of the Baylis and Driver paradigm, the experiments replicated a two-object cost. However, they also showed a large part of the two-object cost to be attributable to space-based factors, though there remained an irreducible cost consistent with 'true' object-based selection.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号