首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   1051篇
  免费   34篇
  国内免费   5篇
  2020年   14篇
  2018年   15篇
  2016年   21篇
  2015年   15篇
  2014年   20篇
  2013年   124篇
  2012年   23篇
  2011年   33篇
  2010年   22篇
  2009年   28篇
  2008年   27篇
  2007年   29篇
  2006年   31篇
  2005年   27篇
  2004年   19篇
  2003年   23篇
  2002年   23篇
  2001年   16篇
  1999年   19篇
  1998年   20篇
  1997年   12篇
  1996年   13篇
  1995年   14篇
  1994年   10篇
  1993年   14篇
  1992年   16篇
  1991年   14篇
  1990年   13篇
  1989年   10篇
  1988年   16篇
  1987年   14篇
  1985年   16篇
  1983年   12篇
  1981年   14篇
  1980年   15篇
  1979年   11篇
  1978年   21篇
  1977年   12篇
  1976年   19篇
  1975年   15篇
  1974年   16篇
  1973年   13篇
  1972年   17篇
  1971年   14篇
  1970年   10篇
  1969年   17篇
  1968年   18篇
  1967年   17篇
  1966年   21篇
  1965年   11篇
排序方式: 共有1090条查询结果,搜索用时 125 毫秒
111.
This paper describes a brief measure of Bales's three SYMLOG dimensions. A sample of 204 undergraduates with a mean age of 24.7 yr., 72.4% of whom were women, completed the usual version of the SYMLOG Trait Rating Form. This could also be scored as if it were a simplified version, omitting some items. The Simplified Measure of the SYMLOG Trait Rating Form correlated fairly highly with the full form--with r between .87 and .89 for the corresponding versions of all three scale dimension scores--making it a suitable substitute for research or illustrative purposes on occasions when respondents' time is of the essence.  相似文献   
112.
Young children experience considerable difficulty in learning their first few color terms. One explanation for this difficulty is that initially they lack a conceptual representation of color sufficiently abstract to support word meaning. This hypothesis, that prior to learning color terms children do not represent color as an abstraction, was tested in two experiments using samples of 25- to 39-month-olds and 20- to 32-month-olds. Children's ability to conceptually represent color and their knowledge of color terms were assessed, and a strong association was found between the ability to make inferences based on color and the comprehension of color words. Children who did not comprehend color terms were unsuccessful at a conceptual task that required them to represent color as a property independent of the particular objects that displayed it. The results suggest that the initial absence of an abstract representation of color contributes to the difficulty that young children encounter when first learning color words.  相似文献   
113.
Intermanual interactions are modulated by task requirements in the course of motor preparation. In particular, amplitude coupling is strong when identical amplitudes are specified concurrently for the 2 hands but relaxed when different amplitudes are specified. Similarly, directional coupling is symmetric when symmetric directions are specified concurrently but turns to parallel when parallel directions are specified. Here, the author investigated whether the modulations of amplitude coupling and directional coupling in the course of motor preparation are independent or interact. Application of the timed-response procedure, which enables one to manipulate the time available for motor preparation, in 9 participants revealed a weakly interactive pattern. Directional coupling tended to be reduced when different rather than same amplitudes were specified concurrently, and amplitude coupling tended to be reduced when parallel rather than symmetric directions were specified concurrently. In general, interactive effects were also apparent in the rates at which directions and amplitudes were specified. Those observations are consistent with the notion that intermanual amplitude and direction interference are mediated by different but partially overlapping neural structures.  相似文献   
114.
In almost all studies of bimanual movements with same and different amplitudes, the difference between amplitudes has been confounded with a difference between endpoint locations. The present authors varied those parameters orthogonally. In addition, they presented target locations on the surface on which the movements were produced (direct cues) and on a monitor (indirect cues). Participants' (N = 12) reaction times were longer when both amplitudes and endpoint locations differed than when they were the same. Intermanual amplitude correlations were reduced whenever 1 of the movement parameters differed for the 2 hands; only when cues were presented on the monitor was the amplitude correlation further reduced when both movement parameters were different. The results indicate that structural constraints on bimanual movements take effect on both amplitudes and endpoint locations. The relative importance of those 2 parameters is largely independent of the type of cue.  相似文献   
115.
116.
117.
An experiment was conducted to investigate the persistence of the effect of "bandwidth knowledge of results (KR)" manipulated during the learning phase of performing a manual force-control task. The experiment consisted of two phases, an acquisition phase with the goal of maintaining 60% maximum force in 30 trials, and a second phase with the objective of maintaining 40% of maximum force in 20 further trials. There were four bandwidths of KR: when performance error exceeded 5, 10, or 15% of the target, and a control group (0% bandwidth). Analysis showed that 5, 10, and 15% bandwidth led to better performance than 0% bandwidth KR at the beginning of the second phase and persisted during the extended trials.  相似文献   
118.
119.
Abstract

We report transmission electron microscopy results on quasicrystalline samples of Al–25 wt% Mn and Al-38 wt% Mn-5 wt% Si melt-spun flakes. The selected-area diffraction patterns (SADPs) from ‘off-axis’ orientations show not only sharp diffraction maxima but also a diffuse ring, invariant of sample thickness, indicating that it does not arise from amorphous surface oxide. Over-exposed SADPs taken from axial orientations also showed the weak diffuse ring. We suggest that our results indicate the presence of disordered material within the icosahedral phase. Models for the icosahedral phase which involve an assembly of icosahedral clusters inherently contain interstitial voids, which may contain disordered material. Thus our results support these models rather than those derived from space-filling filings, or multiple twinning.  相似文献   
120.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号