首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   1243篇
  免费   23篇
  国内免费   5篇
  2018年   20篇
  2016年   17篇
  2014年   14篇
  2013年   119篇
  2012年   20篇
  2011年   39篇
  2010年   27篇
  2009年   31篇
  2008年   40篇
  2007年   37篇
  2006年   41篇
  2005年   28篇
  2004年   22篇
  2003年   24篇
  2002年   30篇
  2001年   18篇
  2000年   15篇
  1999年   19篇
  1998年   21篇
  1997年   14篇
  1996年   16篇
  1994年   12篇
  1993年   16篇
  1992年   27篇
  1991年   18篇
  1990年   15篇
  1989年   20篇
  1988年   27篇
  1987年   21篇
  1985年   16篇
  1984年   14篇
  1983年   23篇
  1982年   13篇
  1981年   14篇
  1980年   15篇
  1979年   21篇
  1978年   18篇
  1977年   15篇
  1976年   23篇
  1975年   20篇
  1974年   23篇
  1973年   12篇
  1972年   22篇
  1971年   16篇
  1970年   12篇
  1969年   14篇
  1968年   21篇
  1967年   26篇
  1966年   27篇
  1965年   12篇
排序方式: 共有1271条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
991.
If one listens to a meaningless syllable that is repeated over and over, he will hear it undergo a variety of changes. These changes are extremely systematic in character and can be described phonetically in terms of reorganizations of the phones constituting the syllable and changes in a restricted set of distinctive features. When a new syllable is presented to a subject after he has listened to a particular syllable that was repeated, he will misreport the new (test) syllable. His misperception of the test syllable is related to the changes occurring in the representation of the original repeated syllable just prior to the presentation of the test syllable.  相似文献   
992.
Two experiments studied whether subjects reported complete adaptation of a taste sensation. Three tasks were chosen based on their use in other laboratories: hand lowering to connote stimulus absence, magnitude estimation, and cross-adaptation of a brief stimulus following a prolonged stimulus. In the first experiment, different groups of 9 subjects received the three different tasks; in the second experiment, all 20 subjects received all three tasks. In both experiments, subjects failed to demonstrate complete taste adaptation in at least 50% of adaptation trials. Response task did affect the likelihood of observing complete taste adaptation.  相似文献   
993.
994.
995.
Identification, discrimination, and match-to-sample paradigms are discussed in the context of a comparative judgment between an external stimulus and a memorial representation. An experiment comparing the paradigms at different levels of stimulus dimensionality is described. The absence of clear-cut interactions of the paradigms with dimensionality differences is offered as support for the contention that the three seemingly diverse procedures may be considered as part of a comparative judgment continuum,with performance differences reflecting increased memorial demands.  相似文献   
996.
Studied the factor structure of the Behavior Problem Checklist using children from a middle to upper-middle-class suburban school system, determined the extent to which factors generated from children in regular classrooms approximated those generated from children in special education, and examined the effects of class placement, sex, and the interactions of these variables on the average rating children receive on the generated factors. One hundred children from 12 classes for the emotionally disturbed or learning disabled and 527 children from regular forst-through seventh-grade classes were rated by their teachers on the Behaviors Problem Checklist. Following a varimax factor rotation, coefficients of factor congruence and analysis of variance were conducted to test the hypotheses. Results indicated that although teacher ratings of special education and normal children are structurally similar, special education children scored higher on all three factors than did their regular class counterparts. Furthermore, almost all percentages of occurance for individual items were higher for special education children. Further research is suggested to deal with a possible expectancy effect in teacher ratings of problem behaviors.  相似文献   
997.
Subjects were shown an arrow inside a rectangle and were asked to decide on either its vertical direction (up vs. downl or its height (high vs. low) as quickly as possible. Whenever vertical direction was criterial, height was irrelevant, and vice versa. In four conditions, the irrelevant dimension (1) did not vary, (2) covaried positively with the criterial dimension, up with high and down with low, (3) covaried negatively with the criterial dimension, up with low and down with high, or (4) varied orthogonally with the criterial dimension. Height and vertical direction satisfied one of W. R. Garner’s prerequisites for “integral” dimensions in that Condition 4 took longer than Condition 1. But Condition 2 was faster than 1, and 3 was slower than 1, a pattern unlike those of other known integral dimensions. The positive correlation in Condition 2 facilitates, and the negative correlation in 3 interferes, it is argued, because height and vertical direction have interpretations with components in common or in conflict. This research was supported in part by Grant MH 20021 from the National lnstitute of Mental Health.  相似文献   
998.
The effect of task instructions on taste adaptation was investigated in two experiments. In the first experiment, three groups of subjects received a 3-min flow of salt solution, over the anterior dorsal tongue surface and periodically gave magnitude estimates of its intensity. Each group had previously received different instructions suggesting the appropriate outcome of the adaptation experiment. Not all subjects showed adaptation, and the instructions had no significant differential effects. In the second experiment, subjects who were practiced in judging pulsatile taste stimuli were instructed to rate the intensity of a continuous salt stimulus as it disappeared, but this produced no increased adaptation. Several possible hypotheses are presented to account for this repeatedly observed failure of many subjects to completely adapt to taste stimuli.  相似文献   
999.
The goal of the study was to define certain of the effective dimensions of visual stimulation in the infant's visual environment by analyzing the well-documented preference for the bullseye over a pattern of horizontal stripes. The stimuli were ten black and white patterns, including the bullseye and the stripes, designed to vary along three dimensions: concentricity, curvilinearity and number of directions within the stimulus. The dependent measure was total fixation time corrected for position bias. The subects, 18 three-month-old infants, were tested three times each in order to present them with the ten stimuli in all possible combinations with positions reversed.All three dimensions were found to be effective in varying degrees. The results were discussed in terms of underlying mechanisms and the infant's ability to process information from a multidimensional stimulus.  相似文献   
1000.
Journal of Philosophical Logic -  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号