全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1438篇 |
免费 | 16篇 |
专业分类
1454篇 |
出版年
2019年 | 16篇 |
2018年 | 19篇 |
2016年 | 16篇 |
2015年 | 20篇 |
2014年 | 19篇 |
2013年 | 147篇 |
2012年 | 33篇 |
2011年 | 51篇 |
2010年 | 33篇 |
2009年 | 31篇 |
2008年 | 42篇 |
2007年 | 44篇 |
2006年 | 45篇 |
2005年 | 43篇 |
2004年 | 33篇 |
2003年 | 37篇 |
2002年 | 36篇 |
2001年 | 16篇 |
2000年 | 25篇 |
1999年 | 27篇 |
1998年 | 26篇 |
1997年 | 14篇 |
1996年 | 22篇 |
1995年 | 21篇 |
1994年 | 14篇 |
1993年 | 15篇 |
1992年 | 26篇 |
1991年 | 16篇 |
1990年 | 19篇 |
1989年 | 14篇 |
1988年 | 22篇 |
1987年 | 21篇 |
1985年 | 26篇 |
1983年 | 15篇 |
1982年 | 16篇 |
1981年 | 14篇 |
1980年 | 22篇 |
1979年 | 19篇 |
1978年 | 19篇 |
1977年 | 13篇 |
1976年 | 29篇 |
1975年 | 23篇 |
1974年 | 22篇 |
1972年 | 24篇 |
1971年 | 14篇 |
1970年 | 15篇 |
1969年 | 15篇 |
1968年 | 27篇 |
1967年 | 24篇 |
1966年 | 26篇 |
排序方式: 共有1454条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
71.
Jescheniak JD Hantsch A Schriefers H 《Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition》2005,31(5):905-920
Speakers are regularly confronted with the choice among lexical alternatives when referring to objects, including basic-level names (e.g., car) and subordinate-level names (e.g., Beetle). Which of these names is eventually selected often depends on contextual factors. The present article reports a series of picture-word interference experiments that explored how the designated target name (basic level vs. subordinate level) and contextual constraints rendering the name alternative either appropriate or inappropriate affect lexical activation and lexical choice. The experimental data demonstrate clear context effects on the eventual lexical choice. However, they also show that alternative nonselected object names are phonologically activated, even if a constraining context makes these alternative names currently inappropriate. 相似文献
72.
73.
Kindling-induced changes in plasticity of the rat amygdala and hippocampus 总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3
下载免费PDF全文

Schubert M Siegmund H Pape HC Albrecht D 《Learning & memory (Cold Spring Harbor, N.Y.)》2005,12(5):520-526
Temporal lobe epilepsy (TLE) is often accompanied by interictal behavioral abnormalities, such as fear and memory impairment. To identify possible underlying substrates, we analyzed long-term synaptic plasticity in two relevant brain regions, the lateral amygdala (LA) and the CA1 region of the hippocampus, in the kindling model of epilepsy. Wistar rats were kindled through daily administration of brief electrical stimulations to the left basolateral nucleus of the amygdala. Field potential recordings were performed in slices obtained from kindled rats 48 h after the last induced seizure, and in slices from sham-implanted and nonimplanted controls. Kindling resulted in a significant impairment of long-term potentiation (LTP) in both the LA and the CA1, the magnitude of which was dependent on the number of prior stage V seizures. Saturation of CA1-LTP, assessed through repeated spaced delivery of high-frequency stimulation, occurred at lower levels in kindled compared to sham-implanted animals, consistent with the hypothesis of reduced capacity of further synaptic strengthening. Furthermore, theta pulse stimulation elicited long-term depression in the amygdala in nonimplanted and sham-implanted controls, whereas the same stimulation protocol stimulation caused LTP in kindled rats. In conclusion, kindling differentially affects the magnitude, saturation, and polarity of LTP in the CA1 and LA, respectively, most likely indicating an activity-dependent mechanism in the context of synaptic metaplasticity. 相似文献
74.
75.
Most 4-year-olds can predict the behavior of a person who wants an object but is mistaken about its location. More difficult is predicting behavior when the person is mistaken about location and wants to avoid the object. We tested between two explanations for children's difficulties with avoidance false belief: the Selection Processing model of inhibitory processing and a General Difficulty account. Children were presented with a false belief task and a control task, in which belief attribution was as difficult as in the false belief task. Predicting behavior in light of the character's desire to avoid the object added more difficulty in the false belief task. This finding is consistent with the Selection Processing model, but not with the General Difficulty account. 相似文献
76.
Core mechanisms in "theory of mind" 总被引:12,自引:0,他引:12
Our ability to understand the thoughts and feelings of other people does not initially develop as a theory but as a mechanism. The "theory of mind" mechanism (ToMM) is part of the core architecture of the human brain, and is specialized for learning about mental states. Impaired development of this mechanism can have drastic effects on social learning, seen most strikingly in the autistic spectrum disorders. ToMM kick-starts belief-desire attribution but effective reasoning about belief contents depends on a process of selection by inhibition. This selection process (SP) develops slowly through the preschool period and well beyond. By modeling the ToMM-SP as mechanisms of selective attention, we have uncovered new empirical phenomena. We propose that early "theory of mind" is a modular-heuristic process of domain-specific learning. 相似文献
77.
78.
Factor analysis was performed on the Beck Depression Inventory with a university sample to examine its potential multidimensionality. A principal components analysis with an oblimin and varimax rotation produced a two-factor solution. These factors were labeled Cognitive-Affective and Physiological and accounted for approximately 39% of the common variance. This finding is consistent with multidimensionality of the inventory and with a similar study of college students. The commonalities of the two studies suggest the reliability (internal consistency) of the Cognitive-Affective and Physiological constructs among "minimally" depressed university samples. 相似文献
79.
Memory for the time of events may benefit from reconstructive, location-based, and distance-based processes, but these processes
are difficult to dissociate with behavioral methods. Neuropsychological research has emphasized the contribution of prefrontal
brain mechanisms to memory for time but has not clearly differentiated location- from distance-based processing. The present
experiment recorded event-related brain potentials (ERPs) while subjects completed two different temporal memory tests, designed
to emphasize either location- or distance-based processing. The subjects’ reports of locationbased versus distance-based strategies
and the reaction time pattern validated our experimental manipulation. Late (800–1,800 msec) frontal ERP effects were related
to location-based processing. The results provide support for a two-process theory of memory for time and suggest that frontal
memory mechanisms are specifically related to reconstructive, location-based processing. 相似文献
80.
Harvey E Danforth JS McKee TE Ulaszek WR Friedman JL 《Journal of attention disorders》2003,7(1):31-42
OBJECTIVE: The present study examined the relation between parental ADHD symptomatology and parent-child behavior among 46 mothers and 26 fathers of ADHD children. RESULTS: Fathers' self-reports of inattention and impulsivity were strongly associated with self-reports of lax parenting both before and after parent training, and with self-reports of overreactivity after parent training. Fathers' impulsivity was also associated with more arguing during audiotaped observations of parent-child interactions prior to parent training. Mothers' self-reports of inattention were modestly associated with self-reports of laxness before and after parent training. Prior to parent training, there were non-linear relations between mothers' inattention and observations of mother-child behavior, with mothers who reported moderate levels of inattention engaging in the most negative parent-child interactions. After parent training, these relations were linear, with the mothers who reported the most inattention engaging in the most negative parent-child interactions. These results were weakened but were generally still significant when parental depression and alcohol use were controlled. 相似文献