首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   1989篇
  免费   34篇
  2023篇
  2020年   35篇
  2019年   33篇
  2018年   45篇
  2017年   40篇
  2016年   41篇
  2015年   37篇
  2014年   39篇
  2013年   213篇
  2012年   66篇
  2011年   74篇
  2010年   52篇
  2009年   53篇
  2008年   78篇
  2007年   67篇
  2006年   61篇
  2005年   49篇
  2004年   54篇
  2003年   59篇
  2002年   50篇
  2001年   28篇
  2000年   41篇
  1999年   37篇
  1998年   33篇
  1997年   33篇
  1996年   21篇
  1995年   15篇
  1994年   20篇
  1993年   20篇
  1992年   24篇
  1991年   25篇
  1990年   17篇
  1989年   16篇
  1988年   20篇
  1987年   24篇
  1986年   24篇
  1985年   23篇
  1984年   24篇
  1983年   27篇
  1982年   21篇
  1979年   32篇
  1978年   18篇
  1977年   23篇
  1976年   36篇
  1975年   25篇
  1974年   22篇
  1973年   21篇
  1972年   19篇
  1971年   18篇
  1970年   16篇
  1967年   16篇
排序方式: 共有2023条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
951.
    
  相似文献   
952.
    
This longitudinal study has followed seventy-six individuals from birth to age 30 using films of the mother-child interaction, psychoanalytically informed interviews of parents and children, and psycho diagnostic testing to assess how the quality of mothering a child receives in the first year of life contributes to his/her subsequent emotional well-being. The thirty-year follow-up of the now adult participants found that those who had received more effective care in infancy in terms of maternal empathy, consistency, control, thoughtfulness, affection and management of aggression had higher-level psychological defence mechanisms as adults than children receiving less effective nurturing-suggesting a process in which the children internalised their mothers' own defence mechanisms. Other measures at 30 years (Global Functioning, Erikson psychosocial attainment, mental representation of security of attachment to parents and presence or absence of a psychiatric diagnosis) did not achieve statistical significance. On the other hand, trauma after infancy and before age 18 provided the strongest correlation with adult outcome: consistent with the theory of the effect of cumulative trauma on psychic functioning, children experiencing two or more adverse circumstances had significantly lower levels of global functioning as adults than those spared multiple traumas. With case examples, the findings illustrate how the effect of pre-verbal experience attenuates over time, and how later influences overlie early life in the course of psychological structuralisation.  相似文献   
953.
    
In recent papers (Seiden, 2004a, 2004b) parallels have been examined between poetry and psychoanalysis in the way language is used to create, extend, transform, and give voice to meaning. What follows is a presentation of work with a child therapy patient which made use of these parallels in employing collaborative poetry as a means to a psychoanalytic end. The parallels suggest several lessons for psychoanalysts. One among them is that interpretation may be less important that we have been taught to think for the working through that is essential in enduring change. A second, as Winnicott (1971a) famously pointed out, is that joy in the process is an important part of the child therapy experience. Copyright © 2007 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.  相似文献   
954.
    
Recent neuroimaging studies show that human rapid-eye-movement (REM) sleep is characterized by a specific pattern of regional brain activity. Although this is usually interpreted in relation to physiological and cellular mechanisms, the specific regional distribution of brain activity during REM sleep might also be linked to specific dream features. Remarkably, several bizarre features of normal dreams have similarities with well-known neuropsychological syndromes after brain damage, such as delusional misidentifications for faces and places. We propose that neuropsychological analysis of dream content might offer new ways of interpreting neuroimaging maps of sleep, and make specific predictions for future neuroimaging studies.  相似文献   
955.
956.
    
Within the context of behavioral economics, the ratio of response requirements to reinforcer magnitude is called unit price. In this investigation, we yoked increases in reinforcer magnitude with increases in intervals of differential reinforcement of other behavior (DRO) to thin DRO intervals to a terminal value.  相似文献   
957.
    
Book reviewed in this article: Juliene G. Lipson and Suzanne L. Dibble, Eds. (2005). Culture & Clinical Care (2nd ed.).  相似文献   
958.
    
The term ‘fundamentalist’ is often used indiscriminately. In this essay, the term is used to refer to movements that insist on strict conformity to a sacred text and to a moral code ostensibly based on it. Such movements often also articulate secular grievances such as nationalistic resentment of foreign domination. To focus only on the fundamentalist dimension of such groups is, in some cases at least, to ignore some of the principal sources of their appeal. One should avoid assuming that moral outrage ostensibly provoked by the violation of traditional religious values is really a mere reflection of nationalistic, ethnic, or other social grievances. But one should also avoid ignoring social grievances when the available evidence suggests that they are in fact relevant.  相似文献   
959.
    
In the current investigation, we evaluated the effects of open and closed economies on the adaptive behavior of 2 individuals with developmental disabilities. Across both types of economy, progressive-ratio (PR) schedules were used in which the number of responses required to obtain reinforcement increased as the session progressed. In closed-economy sessions, participants were able to obtain reinforcement only through interaction with the PR schedule requirements (i.e., more work resulted in more reinforcer access). In open-economy sessions, participants obtained reinforcers by responding on the PR schedule and were given supplemental (free) access to the reinforcers after completion of the session. In general, more responding was associated with the closed economy.  相似文献   
960.
    
This study examined two key issues: (1) whether there were developmental improvements in eyewitness memory performance for children with intellectual disabilities (ID); and (2) whether standardised measures of cognitive ability and suggestibility would relate to eyewitness recall and suggestibility. Children with ID and age‐matched controls (ages 8/9 and 12 years) watched a video of a crime and were asked a range of open‐ended and specific questions about the event in a subsequent interview. Free recall increased between the two age levels for children with and without ID, but at a faster rate for those without ID. For other question types, differences in performance between children with and without ID were far more marked than age differences. Standardised measures of interrogative suggestibility (Gudjonsson Suggestibility Scale, GSS), verbal IQ, non‐verbal IQ, mental age and speed of information processing were related to eyewitness performance. In particular, higher eyewitness recall scores (free recall, non‐leading specific questions) were related to higher scores on the standardised GSS free recall measure; and higher eyewitness suggestibility scores were related to higher scores on the standardised GSS suggestibility measures. Mental age was a better predictor of performance on a range of eyewitness memory question types than verbal or non‐verbal IQ; and speed of information processing showed some relationships with eyewitness performance. Copyright © 2006 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号