全文获取类型
收费全文 | 39513篇 |
免费 | 625篇 |
国内免费 | 2篇 |
专业分类
40140篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 457篇 |
2019年 | 571篇 |
2018年 | 743篇 |
2017年 | 807篇 |
2016年 | 827篇 |
2015年 | 586篇 |
2014年 | 707篇 |
2013年 | 3031篇 |
2012年 | 1287篇 |
2011年 | 1282篇 |
2010年 | 846篇 |
2009年 | 800篇 |
2008年 | 1159篇 |
2007年 | 1169篇 |
2006年 | 1036篇 |
2005年 | 915篇 |
2004年 | 864篇 |
2003年 | 820篇 |
2002年 | 835篇 |
2001年 | 1171篇 |
2000年 | 1120篇 |
1999年 | 880篇 |
1998年 | 394篇 |
1997年 | 393篇 |
1996年 | 357篇 |
1995年 | 362篇 |
1993年 | 360篇 |
1992年 | 744篇 |
1991年 | 661篇 |
1990年 | 710篇 |
1989年 | 612篇 |
1988年 | 653篇 |
1987年 | 608篇 |
1986年 | 612篇 |
1985年 | 558篇 |
1984年 | 512篇 |
1983年 | 491篇 |
1982年 | 357篇 |
1979年 | 587篇 |
1978年 | 410篇 |
1975年 | 467篇 |
1974年 | 506篇 |
1973年 | 541篇 |
1972年 | 409篇 |
1971年 | 400篇 |
1970年 | 361篇 |
1969年 | 405篇 |
1968年 | 475篇 |
1967年 | 437篇 |
1966年 | 360篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
261.
T O Nelson R J Leonesio R S Landwehr L Narens 《Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition》1986,12(2):279-287
Distinctions are drawn between different predictors of an individual's memory performance, with emphasis on the notion of privileged access to idiosyncratic knowledge. Research is reported in which undergraduates attempted to recall the answers to general-information questions, then made feeling-of-knowing judgments on nonrecalled items, and subsequently had a criterion test (relearning, perceptual identification, or one of two versions of recognition). For predicting an individual's criterion performance, the individual's own feeling-of-knowing predictions were intermediate between two kinds of normative predictions: The individual's feeling-of-knowing predictions were more accurate than predictions derived from normative feeling-of-knowing ratings but were less accurate than predictions derived from base-rate item difficulty (normative probabilities of correct recall). Subsidiary analyses showed that factors other than unreliability are responsible for the partial inaccuracy of the individual's feeling of knowing. Ramifications are discussed for possible ways to improve the accuracy of an individual's feeling-of-knowing predictions. 相似文献
262.
263.
RICHARD E. PRICE MICHAEL M. OMIZO VICTORIA L. HAMMETT 《Journal of counseling and development : JCD》1986,65(2):96-97
The authors discuss issues and suggestions relative to counseling clients with AIDS. Several psychosocial issues are presented, and a psycho-educational model of treatment to meet the needs of AIDS clients is described. 相似文献
264.
265.
Literature on the counseling needs of special populations typically has focused on the inadequate treatment those populations receive. Counselor education programs have responded by developing courses that target specific populations such as women, minorities, or disabled people. There are serious drawbacks, however, to this proliferation of specialized courses. In this article the authors discuss those drawbacks and argue that counselor education programs should include an integrated course that facilitates student self-awareness in conjunction with practical experience with a variety of clients. 相似文献
266.
This article is based on a review of recent literature on “computing stress”; that is, on the psychological stress effects
of working with computer systems. Current research into the sources of computer-related stress has tended to concentrate on
those employees who operate Visual Display Units (VDUs), either on a full-time professional basis as word-processor (WP) operators,
data-entry clerks, secretaries, or those dealing with enquiries from the public; or employees who use the VDU irregularly
as part of their other duties. A few studies have looked at specific occupation groups, such as printers (Kalimo & Lepp?nen,
1985), information technology professionals (programers and analysts), process workers, and college students (Sproull, Kiesler,
& Zubrow, 1984). Generally, however, research has been carried out on office workers, more often women, carrying out administrative
tasks. An attempt is made to identify those potential work-related stressors that have been isolated by these studies, and
also included are criticisms of the general methodologies used in many of them.
The terms “visual display unit,” “visual display terminal,” VDU, VDT, and “terminal” are used interchangeably. 相似文献
267.
L Gray R Jahrsdoerfer 《Journal of comparative psychology (Washington, D.C. : 1983)》1986,100(2):91-94
Neonatal ducklings and chickens were tested for responsiveness to a pulsing pure tone that was as similar as possible to the mallard maternal alarm call. It is known that ducklings momentarily cease vocalizing when they hear the alarm call and that chicks do the same when they hear pure tones. The duration of peep suppression can thus be used as a measure of whether subjects of either species heard the stimulus. Chicks might not be as sensitive as ducklings to a mallard alarm call because the signal is less significant to them. An adaptive or staircase procedure was used to estimate absolute thresholds, and group psychometric functions were reconstructed for each species from the trial-by-trial data. Ducklings had lower thresholds than chickens as well as steeper psychometric functions to this stimulus. The results suggest that more sensitive and consistent behavioral responses can be elicited by naturalistic sounds than by more arbitrary acoustic stimuli. 相似文献
268.
Static depiction of movement 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
A number of pictorial devices were compared in order to assess their relative effectiveness in depicting events. Effectiveness, as measured by a rating task, did not seem to be a function of the classification of a device as natural or metaphorical. Rather, it depended on whether a given device highlighted a distinctive aspect of a particular event (running, jumping, or moving), or simply modified the event. This observation was buttressed by the existence in a forced-choice task of a category boundary for depictions of running (pictures with a certain device were seen as running, those without it were not) but not for moving or jumping. Even preschool children displayed some understanding of metaphorical devices. They chose figures with those devices as running faster than the experimental standard. Results are discussed in the context of Kennedy's characterization of pictorial metaphors and Gibson's framework for understanding the relationship between pictorial and environmental information. 相似文献
269.
Margaret L. Signorella Martha Hansen Krupa Wesley Jamison Nancy Lyons 《Sex roles》1986,14(9-10):475-479
Newcombe, Bandura, and Taylor developed a questionnaire measuring participation in space-related activities. The activities were classified as masculine, feminine, or neutral in gender stereotyping. A short version of this questionnaire was developed based on an item analysis of the responses by 485 introductory psychology students (236 women and 249 men). The 10 masculine, 10 feminine, and 10 neutral items chosen for the short form were given to 60 subjects (28 women and 32 men), along with a measure of spatial performance (Piaget's water-level task). Reliabilities (by coefficient alpha) were .79, .77, and .75, for the masculine, feminine, and neutral activity subscales, respectively. As with the original scale, women had a greater preference than men for feminine activities, men had a greater preference than women for masculine activities, and women and men had an equal preference for neutral activities. Masculine activity preference was associated with better water-level performance, with the relationship significantly stronger for women (r=.50) than for men (r=.10). Uses for the questionnaire in research on sex-related differences in spatial performance are suggested. 相似文献
270.
This study examined the relationships among support types (i.e., emotional, instrumental, and nonintimate social participation), gender, sex role orientation, and stress level among college undergraduates (N=253). Dependent variables included need, perceived availability, and satisfaction with support. Sex differences were found only in emotional support, with men reporting less need, perceived availability, and marginally less satisfaction than women. The only gender by sex role interaction was on need for emotional support. Traditional sex-typed men reported less need than traditional women, whereas there was no difference between androgynous men and women. While sex differences do exist for emotional support, the effects of sex role orientation on perceptions of social support appears to be somewhat circumscribed. 相似文献