首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   34453篇
  免费   1453篇
  国内免费   30篇
  2020年   412篇
  2019年   501篇
  2018年   716篇
  2017年   647篇
  2016年   746篇
  2015年   516篇
  2014年   595篇
  2013年   2765篇
  2012年   1132篇
  2011年   1123篇
  2010年   684篇
  2009年   741篇
  2008年   959篇
  2007年   981篇
  2006年   867篇
  2005年   739篇
  2004年   737篇
  2003年   671篇
  2002年   690篇
  2001年   1164篇
  2000年   1166篇
  1999年   852篇
  1998年   356篇
  1997年   346篇
  1995年   331篇
  1992年   674篇
  1991年   647篇
  1990年   611篇
  1989年   587篇
  1988年   590篇
  1987年   516篇
  1986年   505篇
  1985年   564篇
  1984年   463篇
  1983年   425篇
  1982年   333篇
  1979年   504篇
  1978年   349篇
  1977年   328篇
  1976年   353篇
  1975年   383篇
  1974年   459篇
  1973年   486篇
  1972年   415篇
  1971年   368篇
  1970年   339篇
  1969年   353篇
  1968年   432篇
  1967年   390篇
  1966年   372篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
861.
An experiment was performed which demonstrated a preference in hooded rats for vertical rather than horizontal striatums. It was demonstrated that experience in discriminating between horizontal and vertical striations facilitates subsequent discrimination between squares and circles, regardless of which type of striation was positive in initial training. This finding has implications for theories of shape recognition, which are discussed.  相似文献   
862.
863.
864.
865.
Two recent experiments (Moray, 1959; Oswald, Taylor and Treisman, 1960) have shown that in dichotic listening, and in sleep one's own name seems to be responded to selectively. We have shown for 10 subjects, each listening binaurally to the same recording of a list of their names repeated at random 10 times, and masked by noise, that the subject's own name had a significantly lower threshold than other names. The effect we found was not significantly different from the one found by Oswald et al. during sleep and by Moray for the dichotic situation.

These three experiments taken together suggest that the same pattern-analysing mechanism is involved in normal listening, dichotic listening to the rejected message, and in discrimination during sleep.  相似文献   
866.
867.
On the failure to eliminate hypotheses in a conceptual task   总被引:13,自引:0,他引:13  
This investigation examines the extent to which intelligent young adults seek (i) confirming evidence alone (enumerative induction) or (ii) confirming and discontinuing evidence (eliminative induction), in order to draw conclusions in a simple conceptual task. The experiment is designed so that use of confirming evidence alone will almost certainly lead to erroneous conclusions because (i) the correct concept is entailed by many more obvious ones, and (ii) the universe of possible instances (numbers) is infinite.

Six out of 29 subjects reached the correct conclusion without previous incorrect ones, 13 reached one incorrect conclusion, nine reached two or more incorrect conclusions, and one reached no conclusion. The results showed that those subjects, who reached two or more incorrect conclusions, were unable, or unwilling to test their hypotheses. The implications are discussed in relation to scientific thinking.  相似文献   
868.
869.
An exposition of a calculus for empirical classes (CEC), one of the few attempts by logicians to deal with the problem of constructs and indicators, is presented. The CEC provides the groundwork for a formal structure for the situation in which individuals have a degree of membership in various classes rather than having either membership or nonmembership—a situation nearly always true in empirical research. The CEC is presented and its relation to various social science concepts is mentioned. An application of the CEC model to latent structure analysis (LSA) suggests alternatives to the local independence assumption including one called the local scale assumption, which has a close relation to a Guttman scale.  相似文献   
870.
The processing of positive and negative information   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
An affirmative statement which is known to be false and the complementary negative statement which is known to be true, provide the same information, i.e. that something is not the case. Similarly, an affirmative statement which is known to be true and the complementary negative statement which is known to be false, both imply that something is the case. (If P is false, not-P is true and if P is true, not-P is false.) Hence there are four kinds of statement (“conditions”): true affirmatives, false affirmatives, true negativee and false negatives, but only two kinds of information: positive and negative.

This experiment investigates the times taken to process information presented in these ways. The task was to select two alternative words which would make affirmative or negative conjunctive statements agree or conflict with given situations. The four conditions were presented six times in different serial orders, so that each occurred once in every block of four trials. The mean response times were: true affirmatives 8-99 sec, false affirmatives 11 19 sec., true negatives 12-58 sec, false negatives 15-17 sec. This order was the same at each of the six presentations of the conditions, the differences being significant at the 0 001 level in each case. There was a pronounced decline in errors (without knowledge of results) for three of the conditions. These results are discussed in relation to (i) the assumption of a positive set, established through a long learning process; (ii) the inferential nature of negative information in relation to experience; and (iii) the possible emotional effects of negative terms.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号