全文获取类型
收费全文 | 195篇 |
免费 | 6篇 |
专业分类
201篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 4篇 |
2023年 | 2篇 |
2022年 | 1篇 |
2021年 | 5篇 |
2020年 | 9篇 |
2019年 | 4篇 |
2018年 | 10篇 |
2017年 | 12篇 |
2016年 | 13篇 |
2015年 | 5篇 |
2014年 | 10篇 |
2013年 | 31篇 |
2012年 | 12篇 |
2011年 | 9篇 |
2010年 | 7篇 |
2009年 | 9篇 |
2008年 | 10篇 |
2007年 | 7篇 |
2006年 | 6篇 |
2005年 | 6篇 |
2004年 | 6篇 |
2003年 | 5篇 |
2002年 | 1篇 |
2001年 | 2篇 |
2000年 | 4篇 |
1999年 | 3篇 |
1998年 | 2篇 |
1996年 | 1篇 |
1992年 | 1篇 |
1975年 | 1篇 |
1967年 | 1篇 |
1962年 | 2篇 |
排序方式: 共有201条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
11.
12.
Nicolas Gauvrit Henrik Singmann Fernando Soler-Toscano Hector Zenil 《Behavior research methods》2016,48(1):314-329
Kolmogorov-Chaitin complexity has long been believed to be impossible to approximate when it comes to short sequences (e.g. of length 5-50). However, with the newly developed coding theorem method the complexity of strings of length 2-11 can now be numerically estimated. We present the theoretical basis of algorithmic complexity for short strings (ACSS) and describe an R-package providing functions based on ACSS that will cover psychologists’ needs and improve upon previous methods in three ways: (1) ACSS is now available not only for binary strings, but for strings based on up to 9 different symbols, (2) ACSS no longer requires time-consuming computing, and (3) a new approach based on ACSS gives access to an estimation of the complexity of strings of any length. Finally, three illustrative examples show how these tools can be applied to psychology. 相似文献
13.
The goal of our study was to localize the source of the stronger Stroop interference effect found in morphosyllabic readers
as compared with alphabetic readers. Twenty-three Chinese and 24 German undergraduate students were tested in a Stroop paradigm
with the following stimuli: color patches, colorneutral words (e.g.,friend printed in yellow), incongruent color-associated words (e.g.,blood printed in blue), and incongruent color words (e.g.,yellow printed in blue). Results revealed no differences in German and Chinese students’ response times to color patches. Chinese
participants, however, showed longer color naming latencies for neutral words as well as for color words and color-related
words. No differences between German and Chinese participants were found when print color latencies for neutral words were
subtracted from print color latencies for color words and color-related words. This result does not support theories which
suggest that for morphosyllabic readers there is a direct route from orthography to the semantics of a word. We rather argue,
with reference to dual route models of reading, that access from print to phonology is faster for morphosyllabic than for
alphabetic readers, and therefore interference caused by conflicting phonologies of color name and written word will be stronger
in Chinese readers than in German readers. 相似文献
14.
Dries Trippas David Kellen Henrik Singmann Gordon Pennycook Derek J. Koehler Jonathan A. Fugelsang Chad Dubé 《Psychonomic bulletin & review》2018,25(6):2141-2174
The belief-bias effect is one of the most-studied biases in reasoning. A recent study of the phenomenon using the signal detection theory (SDT) model called into question all theoretical accounts of belief bias by demonstrating that belief-based differences in the ability to discriminate between valid and invalid syllogisms may be an artifact stemming from the use of inappropriate linear measurement models such as analysis of variance (Dube et al., Psychological Review, 117(3), 831–863, 2010). The discrepancy between Dube et al.’s, Psychological Review, 117(3), 831–863 (2010) results and the previous three decades of work, together with former’s methodological criticisms suggests the need to revisit earlier results, this time collecting confidence-rating responses. Using a hierarchical Bayesian meta-analysis, we reanalyzed a corpus of 22 confidence-rating studies (N =?993). The results indicated that extensive replications using confidence-rating data are unnecessary as the observed receiver operating characteristic functions are not systematically asymmetric. These results were subsequently corroborated by a novel experimental design based on SDT’s generalized area theorem. Although the meta-analysis confirms that believability does not influence discriminability unconditionally, it also confirmed previous results that factors such as individual differences mediate the effect. The main point is that data from previous and future studies can be safely analyzed using appropriate hierarchical methods that do not require confidence ratings. More generally, our results set a new standard for analyzing data and evaluating theories in reasoning. Important methodological and theoretical considerations for future work on belief bias and related domains are discussed. 相似文献
15.
Research has documented that children and adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) are at increased
risk of substance use problems. Few studies, however, have focused on early-onset substance use. This study therefore investigated
how the two symptom dimensions of ADHD (hyperactivity/impulsivity and inattention) are associated with early-onset substance
use, the role of persistent ADHD for the association, and to what extent the association is influenced by genetic and environmental
factors. Twins (1,480 pairs) in the Swedish Twin Study of Child and Adolescent Development were followed from childhood to
adolescence. ADHD symptoms were measured at age 8–9 and age 13–14 via parent-report, whereas substance use was assessed at
age 13–14 via self-report. Results revealed that hyperactive/impulsive symptoms predicted early-onset “sometimes” tobacco
use (adjusted odds ratios, 1.12, for one symptom count), controlling for inattentive symptoms and conduct problem behaviors.
There is no independent effect of inattentive symptoms on early-onset substance use. Children with persistent hyperactivity/impulsivity
(defined as scoring above the 75th percentile at both time points) had a pronounced risk of both early-onset tobacco and alcohol
use (adjusted odds ratios from 1.86 to 3.35, compared to the reference group). The associations between hyperactivity/impulsivity
and early-onset substance use were primarily influenced by genetic factors. Our results indicated that hyperactivity/impulsivity,
but not inattention, is an important early predictor for early-onset substance use, and a shared genetic susceptibility is
suggested to explain this association. 相似文献
16.
Niels Henrik Gregersen 《Dialog》2016,55(3):247-261
This article has the twofold aim of bringing the Christology of St. Bonaventure into dialogue with contemporary attempts to formulate an informational worldview, and with the recent theological proposal of deep incarnation. It is argued that even though Bonaventure is a pre‐modern theologian, he anticipates central aspects of a contemporary informational worldview based on differential information, structural information, and semantic information. Moreover, the article explores Bonaventure's unique combination of a “high” exemplarist Christology with a “low” Franciscan view of the humility of Christ. While Bonaventure shares seminal concerns of deep incarnation, his strong view of divine perfection prohibits him from assuming that the divinity of Christ embraces the full gamut of human emotions and anxieties related to the future. 相似文献
17.
18.
19.
The Journal of Value Inquiry - 相似文献
20.
Odin Hjemdal Roger Hagen Stian Solem Henrik Nordahl Leif Edward Ottesen Kennair Truls Ryum Hans M. Nordahl Adrian Wells 《Cognitive and behavioral practice》2017,24(3):312-318
This open trial investigated the transdiagnostic effects of metacognitive therapy (MCT) in patients with severe major depressive disorder and comorbid psychiatric disorder. Ten patients were treated with MCT over 10 sessions and were assessed with measures of depression, anxiety, rumination, and metacognitions at pre- and posttreatment and at 6 months follow-up. None of the patients were diagnosed as depressed at posttreatment, and of the initial 21 total diagnoses at pretreatment only 3 diagnoses remained at postintervention. The effect sizes were large for symptoms of depression, rumination, and worry. At 6 months follow-up standardized recovery criteria on the BDI showed that 70% were recovered, 20% improved, and 10% unchanged. The results indicate that MCT was associated with high rates of transdiagnostic improvement. 相似文献