首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   4011篇
  免费   203篇
  2024年   7篇
  2023年   42篇
  2022年   41篇
  2021年   75篇
  2020年   82篇
  2019年   114篇
  2018年   153篇
  2017年   151篇
  2016年   167篇
  2015年   118篇
  2014年   135篇
  2013年   478篇
  2012年   212篇
  2011年   238篇
  2010年   139篇
  2009年   146篇
  2008年   200篇
  2007年   162篇
  2006年   147篇
  2005年   117篇
  2004年   130篇
  2003年   131篇
  2002年   105篇
  2001年   80篇
  2000年   89篇
  1999年   63篇
  1998年   44篇
  1997年   41篇
  1996年   26篇
  1995年   28篇
  1994年   27篇
  1993年   21篇
  1992年   46篇
  1991年   33篇
  1990年   50篇
  1989年   50篇
  1988年   39篇
  1987年   26篇
  1986年   30篇
  1985年   31篇
  1984年   24篇
  1983年   18篇
  1982年   18篇
  1981年   10篇
  1980年   8篇
  1979年   16篇
  1978年   12篇
  1974年   7篇
  1972年   12篇
  1938年   6篇
排序方式: 共有4214条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
901.
In the modal literature various notions of completeness have been studied for normal modal logics. Four of these are defined here, viz. (plain) completeness, first-order completeness, canonicity and possession of the finite model property — and their connections are studied. Up to one important exception, all possible inclusion relations are either proved or disproved. Hopefully, this helps to establish some order in the jungle of concepts concerning modal logics. In the course of the exposition, the interesting properties of first-order definability and preservation under ultrafilter extensions are introduced and studied as well.  相似文献   
902.
Leeuwenberg's coding system for the representation of line pattern interpretations is tentatively applied to the interpretation of complex line patterns, that is, line patterns with intersecting lines. Perceptually preferred interpretations of complex line patterns can only be predicted correctly if two requirements are met: (a) The codes of line patterns should contain information about the objects represented by the line patterns, not about the line patterns as drawings; and (b) effects of the gestalt law of good continuation have to be taken into account.  相似文献   
903.
In this experimental study, individual risk taking for self and others was observed for three experimental situations (the choice dilemma situations of Kogan and Wallach, 1964, the Pruitt and Teger gambling situation, 1969, and a simple one-trial gamble game). The results of the present study suggest that one takes as much risk for oneself as for somebody else. An attempt was made to determine to what degree the results could be explained by properties of the experimental situations.  相似文献   
904.
905.
906.
In the experiments described in this article we investigated the control of stiffness of the effector system in relation to parameters of the movement. If the velocity of a movement is chosen, it appears that at the same time the stiffness of the effector system is set to a preferred value. By changing the instruction given to the subject it is possible to change the relationship between velocity and stiffness without changing the kinematic parameters of the movement.Finally, the changes of stiffness and velocity in a learning process are studied. In conformity with what might be expected on intuitive grounds, it is found that velocity increases spontaneously during learning while stiffness decreases. The results are discussed in connection with the generation of motor programmes.  相似文献   
907.
Vertical letter pairs were presented randomly in the left and right visual hemifields in a physical identity match and name identity match condition. The reaction times showed a right visual field superiority for name matches, and a left visual field superiority for physical matches. Event-related potentials to letter pairs showed a sequence of three waves: a negative wave (N2, around 270 msec), a positive wave (P3, around 500 msec), and a broad positive slow wave (SW, around 600-700 msec), respectively. P3 and SW amplitudes were consistently larger at the left hemisphere than at the right hemisphere, regardless of the field of stimulation. At both hemispheres, N2 waves were always larger to stimuli presented in the visual field contralateral to a hemisphere than stimuli presented in the visual field ipsilateral to a hemisphere. The positive waves (P3, SW) showed the opposite pattern: smaller amplitudes to stimuli that were presented contralaterally than stimuli that were presented ipsilaterally to a given hemisphere. These results were attributed to a shift in sustained negativity on the directly stimulated hemisphere, relative to the indirectly stimulated hemisphere, reflecting either sensory at attentional processes in the posterior cerebral hemispheres.  相似文献   
908.
909.
Contemporary historians of logic tend to credit Bernard Bolzano with the invention of the semantic notion, of consequence, a full century before Tarski. Nevertheless, Bolzano's work played no significant rôle in the genesis of modern logical semantics. The purpose of this paper is to point out three highly original, and still quite relevant themes in Bolzano's work, being a systematic study of possible types of inference, of consistency, as well as their meta-theory. There are certain analogies with Tarski's concerns here, although the main thrust seems to be different, both philosophically and technically. Thus, if only obliquely, we also provide some additional historical perspective on Tarski's achievement.  相似文献   
910.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号