全文获取类型
收费全文 | 17839篇 |
免费 | 370篇 |
国内免费 | 7篇 |
专业分类
18216篇 |
出版年
2021年 | 139篇 |
2020年 | 254篇 |
2019年 | 255篇 |
2018年 | 371篇 |
2017年 | 375篇 |
2016年 | 390篇 |
2015年 | 293篇 |
2014年 | 323篇 |
2013年 | 1373篇 |
2012年 | 586篇 |
2011年 | 670篇 |
2010年 | 385篇 |
2009年 | 361篇 |
2008年 | 491篇 |
2007年 | 473篇 |
2006年 | 406篇 |
2005年 | 346篇 |
2004年 | 348篇 |
2003年 | 324篇 |
2002年 | 329篇 |
2001年 | 700篇 |
2000年 | 707篇 |
1999年 | 464篇 |
1998年 | 183篇 |
1997年 | 177篇 |
1996年 | 143篇 |
1995年 | 138篇 |
1993年 | 141篇 |
1992年 | 361篇 |
1991年 | 323篇 |
1990年 | 347篇 |
1989年 | 335篇 |
1988年 | 319篇 |
1987年 | 291篇 |
1986年 | 310篇 |
1985年 | 301篇 |
1984年 | 236篇 |
1983年 | 202篇 |
1979年 | 196篇 |
1978年 | 165篇 |
1975年 | 190篇 |
1974年 | 246篇 |
1973年 | 199篇 |
1972年 | 204篇 |
1971年 | 169篇 |
1970年 | 151篇 |
1969年 | 198篇 |
1968年 | 202篇 |
1967年 | 161篇 |
1966年 | 182篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
311.
K Kilborn 《Brain and language》1991,41(2):275-288
The traditional clinical picture for English nonfluent aphasics has generally presented the deficit as one of total loss of control over grammatical morphology, with some sparing of word order. This is at odds with recent research involving nonfluent aphasic speakers of highly inflected languages, which has shown that agrammatic performance is characterized by morphological substitution rather than omission errors. If the deficit associated with focal brain damage cannot be adequately accounted for in syndrome-specific ways, we may need to look for language-specific processing explanations. One such explanation has to do with language-specific response to global processing difficulty. The current experiment is designed to study the effects of a stress-related limitation on morphological processing. Normal speakers of a language with a relatively rich morphological system (German) are compared with those of a comparatively impoverished system (English) on different forms of a sentence comprehension task. In one form, "clean" stimuli permit full reliance on all available cues to meaning in each language. In another test, a low-level noise mask partially obscured the stimulus sentences. English speakers, who rely almost exclusively on word order cues, were not affected by the noise manipulation. German speakers relied heavily on morphological and semantic information rather than on word order under "clean" conditions. However, under noise Germans made significantly less use of grammatical morphology, with a trend toward compensatory reliance on word order. The results indicate that a global reduction in processing capacity can affect some aspects of language more than others and suggest that such factors must be taken into account in trying to understand specific impairment of morphology in aphasia. 相似文献
312.
L Hochhaus K M Marohn 《Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance》1991,17(2):422-432
A visual prime succeeded by a brief target produces a paradox. Namely, target repetition yields poorer identification accuracy and shorter duration judgments than unrelated prime-target pairs. Experiment 1 manipulated stimulus onset asynchrony to learn when repetition blindness is maximized. Experiments 2 and 3 manipulated expectancy of repetitions through changes in the proportion of repeated trials and instructions, respectively. Results indicate that repetition blindness is influenced by subject strategies and that the change is not mediated by response bias. Experiment 4 showed that increasing the spatial distance between prime and target reduced but did not eliminate repetition blindness. The current data support joint explanation of repetition blindness in terms of perceptual capture (prime-target fusion) and token individuation failure (problems in encoding episodic reoccurrences of an event). 相似文献
313.
The detectability of a target pattern presented briefly with a number of similar nontarget patterns varies as a function of the spatial location of the target. Previous work attributes these detectability gradients to a visual search process--a non-eye movement serial scan--that examines a decaying neural representation of the image. (Heron, 1957; Efron, Yund, & Nichols, 1987, 1990a,b,c; Yund, Efron, & Nichols, 1990a,b,c). The results reported in the companion paper (Ostrosky-Solis, Efron, & Yund, 1991) indicated that literacy did not affect overall performance levels but did influence scanning behavior: "...reading, or learning to read, caused the scanning mechanisms of literate subjects to adopt more consistent scan paths, from subject to subject, than they would have adopted without this reading experience." The purpose of the present experiment was to determine the effect on this scanning mechanism, if any, of an entirely different type of visual experience--the high-speed visual processing required of tennis players. Unlike reading which requires the linguistic interpretation of a highly structured visual input, tennis skill requires rapid target detection and tracking in three-dimensional visual space as well as large scale visual-motor coordination. As in the previous experiments, subjects were required to detect a vertical stripe pattern among a number of similar non-target patterns. The experiment was performed on a group of 52 tennis players and on an age- and sex-matched group of 52 non-tennis players. The overall accuracy of target detection was greater among the tennis players than among the non-tennis players and, of more interest, there was a significant difference in the detectability gradients. The detection advantage of the tennis group seemed to reach its maximum in the first half of the scan and then to deteriorate as the scan proceeded. These results indicate that visual experience other than reading can affect the habitual activity of the scanning mechanism. 相似文献
314.
Maximizing the potential of worksites for smoking intervention remains elusive. We hypothesized that long-term effectiveness of group intervention would be enhanced when offered within an "enriched milieu" (full program) compared with relative isolation (group only). The data failed to support the hypothesis. Although sustained abstinence rates were higher at full-program sites (50%) than at group-only sites (44%), the difference did not achieve statistical significance. Initial (35% vs. 47%) and 12-month (18% vs. 22%) quit rates at full-program and group-only sites also failed to demonstrate the "benefit" of the "enriched milieu." 相似文献
315.
The purpose of this study was to investigate the effect of cosmetics use on attributions concerning the likelihood of provoking sexual harassment and of being sexually harassed. Subjects were 85 female and 76 male undergraduate volunteers. The study was a 3×2 between-subjects design with three levels of cosmetics use (heavy, moderate, no cosmetics) and two levels of sex of subject (male, female). Each subject viewed one of three colored photographs of a model wearing either heavy, moderate, or no cosmetics, and then indicated how likely the model was to provoke sexual harassment and to be sexually harassed. Data were analyzed using analyses of variance and the Newman-Keuls test. When the model wore heavy cosmetics, she was rated as more likely to provoke sexual harassment than when she wore moderate cosmetics. Similarly, when the model wore moderate cosmetics, she was rated as significantly more likely to provoke sexual harassment than when she was not wearing cosmetics. When the model wore either heavy or moderate cosmetics, she was also rated as more likely to be sexually harassed than when she did not wear cosmetics. In addition, male subjects rated the model as more likely to provoke and to be sexually harassed than did female subjects. Results are discussed in terms of sex role spillover. 相似文献
316.
R K Young D D Thiessen 《Journal of comparative psychology (Washington, D.C. : 1983)》1991,105(4):340-344
Very few behavioral patterns generalize across human and nonhuman species. We describe washing, drying, and anointing sequences in humans (Homo sapiens) that may have analogies with other species. The rank ordering of washing, drying, and anointing body parts was obtained over 3 successive days for 37 men and 60 women. Variation in rank ordering of body parts was nonrandom, and a cephalocaudal progression was evident for each behavior. Reliability of the behaviors as well as the correlations across rankings were highly significant, which indicates a generalized cephalocaudal progression for all 3 behaviors. Women's anointing was most variable, which suggests a more specific function. The cephalocaudal action pattern described for humans is similar to that for Mongolian gerbils and laboratory rats. Cross-species functions, such as these, may add to our understanding of common developmental and learning processes. 相似文献
317.
The present study examined the differences in internal and external locus of control and imposter phenomenon among persons
with alcoholic and non-alcoholic parents. The subjects were 48 college students and 21 members of local Adult Children of
Alcoholic groups. The students were further divided into those with alcoholic parents and non-alcoholic parents. The subjects
were administered the following three instruments: the Nowicki-Strickland Internal-External Control Scale for Adults (Nowicki
& Duke, 1973), the Imposter Test (Chance & O’Toole, 1987), and a family history survey. Analysis of locus of control scores
revealed a highly significant (p<.01) difference for externality, with Adult Children being most external and students with
non-alcoholic parents least external. Similarly, internality scores differed significantly (p<.0001), with Adult Children
being least, and the non-alcoholic group being most internally oriented. Analysis of variance for imposter phenomenon revealed
a significant (p<.05) difference among the three groups, with the Adult Children of Alcoholics group having the highest scores
and students with non-alcoholic parents having the lowest. These findings are consistent with the idea that parental alcoholism
interferes with nurturing and consistent reinforcement, and with the development of personal adaptability. 相似文献
318.
JAMES A. FOOS ALLEN J. OTTENS LARRY K. HILL 《Journal of counseling and development : JCD》1991,69(4):332-336
The mental health service delivery system is experiencing a transformation attendant to rapidly developing cost containment mechanisms. This article reviews various aspects of the new managed mental health care systems and details the gains and losses the various mental health service provider professions may experience depending upon how each responds to the changing demands of alternate service delivery systems. Recommendations are presented regarding how counselors can establish a solid base in the new managed mental health care system. 相似文献
319.
Thirty community-dwelling, moderately depressed elderly were randomly assigned to 1 of 3 interventions: experimenter-accompanied exercise in the form of walking, a social contact control condition, and a wait-list control. Exercise and social contact both resulted in significant reductions in both the total and the psychological subscale of the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI). The exercise condition, however, unlike the control conditions, resulted in decreased somatic symptoms of the BDI. These results indicate that, at least in the short term, exercise has a broader effect compared with control conditions in reducing depressive symptoms in the moderately depressed elderly. 相似文献
320.
When was the last time you saw a job application with no spelling or grammatical errors? Basic skill deficiencies among people at all levels of business are forcing companies to develop innovative ways of compensating for and correcting the problem. What follows is a summary of literacy training strategies and suggestions for implementation. 相似文献