全文获取类型
收费全文 | 4003篇 |
免费 | 52篇 |
专业分类
4055篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 35篇 |
2023年 | 41篇 |
2022年 | 42篇 |
2021年 | 75篇 |
2020年 | 84篇 |
2019年 | 116篇 |
2018年 | 152篇 |
2017年 | 151篇 |
2016年 | 168篇 |
2015年 | 117篇 |
2014年 | 135篇 |
2013年 | 463篇 |
2012年 | 206篇 |
2011年 | 237篇 |
2010年 | 135篇 |
2009年 | 138篇 |
2008年 | 193篇 |
2007年 | 153篇 |
2006年 | 133篇 |
2005年 | 103篇 |
2004年 | 124篇 |
2003年 | 126篇 |
2002年 | 98篇 |
2001年 | 75篇 |
2000年 | 82篇 |
1999年 | 56篇 |
1998年 | 41篇 |
1997年 | 37篇 |
1996年 | 21篇 |
1995年 | 24篇 |
1994年 | 21篇 |
1993年 | 14篇 |
1992年 | 37篇 |
1991年 | 26篇 |
1990年 | 45篇 |
1989年 | 44篇 |
1988年 | 37篇 |
1987年 | 21篇 |
1986年 | 30篇 |
1985年 | 29篇 |
1984年 | 22篇 |
1983年 | 18篇 |
1982年 | 16篇 |
1981年 | 10篇 |
1980年 | 8篇 |
1979年 | 16篇 |
1978年 | 11篇 |
1972年 | 11篇 |
1966年 | 6篇 |
1938年 | 6篇 |
排序方式: 共有4055条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
211.
Emmy van Deurzen 《欧洲心理治疗、咨询与健康杂志》2013,15(3):488-490
David Aldridge (1997) Suicide: the Tragedy of Hopelessness, London: Jessica Kingsley, $15.95 相似文献
212.
213.
Karen van der Zee Jan Pieter van Oudenhoven Joseph G. Ponterotto Alexander W. Fietzer 《Journal of personality assessment》2013,95(1):118-124
This study reports on the development of the Multicultural Personality Questionnaire–Short Form among 511 participants. Using a split-sample scale validation design, Study 1 (N = 260) employed a principal component analysis and rigorous item selection criteria to extract a 40-item short form (MPQ–SF) from the original 91-item Multicultural Personality Questionnaire (MPQ; van der Zee & van Oudenhoven, 2000, 2001). In Study 2 (N = 251), the MPQ–SF was subjected to confirmatory factor analysis and resulted in a reasonably good fit to the data (comparative fit index = .94; root mean squared error of approximation = .066). Satisfactory coefficient alphas and high correlations with the original scales were found. Moreover, relationships with related scales were largely in the predicted direction. Specific directions for follow-up research are posited. 相似文献
214.
Eveline A. Crone Silvia A. Bunge Heleen Latenstein Maurits W. van der Molen 《Child neuropsychology》2013,19(3):245-263
On a gambling task that models real-life decision making, children between ages 7 and 12 perform like patients with bilateral lesions of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (VMPFC), opting for choices that yield high immediate gains in spite of higher future losses (Crone &; Van der Molen, 2004). The current study set out to characterize developmental changes in decision making by varying task complexity and punishment frequency. Three age groups (7–9 years, 10–12 years, 13–15 years) performed two versions of a computerized variant of the original Iowa gambling task. Task complexity was manipulated by varying the number of choices participants could make. Punishment frequency was manipulated by varying the frequency of delayed punishment. Results showed a developmental increase in the sensitivity to future consequences, which was present only when the punishment was presented infrequently. These results could not be explained by differential sensitivity to task complexity, hypersensitivity to reward, or failure to switch response set after receiving punishment. There was a general pattern of boys outperforming girls by making more advantageous choices over the course of the task. In conclusion, 7–12-year-old children—like VMPFC patients—appear myopic about the future except when the potential for future punishment is high. 相似文献
215.
Yoshito Kawabata Lenneke R.A. Alink Wan-Ling Tseng Marinus H. van IJzendoorn Nicki R. Crick 《Developmental Review》2011,31(4):240-278
How does parenting affect relational aggression in children? The goal of the present series of meta-analyses based on 48 studies (28,097 children) was to analyze and integrate the findings on the associations between various types of parenting behaviors and relational aggression, and to identify potential substantive and methodological factors that may moderate these associations. To distinguish between different parenting strategies, experts sorted the parenting measures used in the studies into 10 groups. Results of a multiple correspondence analysis revealed four separate clusters: Positive parenting, psychologically controlling parenting, negative/harsh parenting, and uninvolved parenting. The meta-analyses demonstrated that more positive parenting was associated with less relational aggression (combined effect sizes r = −.06, p < .05, for mothers, r = −.08, p < .01, for fathers). More harsh parenting (combined effect sizes r = .11, p < .01, for mothers, r = .12, p < .01, for fathers) and more uninvolved parenting (combined effect sizes r = .07, p < .01, for mothers, absent for fathers) were associated with increased relational aggression. Paternal psychologically controlling parenting was positively related to relational aggression (r = .05, p < .01), whereas maternal psychologically controlling parenting was not (combined effect sizes r = .04, p = .09). The effect of several moderators is discussed. The findings of this study suggest that dimensions of positive and negative parenting behaviors of mothers and fathers are associated with children’s relational aggression and that these associations are – in case of fathers – contingent upon a number of sampling and procedural characteristics. 相似文献
216.
Florien M. Cramwinckel Eric van Dijk Daan Scheepers Kees van den Bos 《Journal of experimental social psychology》2013,49(6):1049-1058
We investigated how people respond to moral threats and the consequences this has for one's moral self-concept. In two experiments, participants first tasted a sausage and were then confronted with a bogus participant who had refused to taste the sausage because of moral or non-moral reasons. People disliked the moral refuser more than the non-moral refuser. The self-threatening effect of having one's morals questioned was also reflected in specific patterns of cardiovascular responses and negatively affected participants' self-evaluations. We further show that the negative effects of a moral threat can be prevented by a simple intervention of physical cleansing: Participants who had cleansed their hands before being confronted with a moral refuser did not show the negative effects on self- and refuser evaluations. Importantly, the protective effects of physical cleansing were most pronounced for people with a strong moral identity. Taken together, these results underline the importance of one's self-concept when confronted with a moral refuser, and introduce an effective intervention to prevent these negative consequences. 相似文献
217.
Marij van Strien 《Journal for General Philosophy of Science》2014,45(1):167-185
The recent discovery of an indeterministic system in classical mechanics, the Norton dome, has shown that answering the question whether classical mechanics is deterministic can be a complicated matter. In this paper I show that indeterministic systems similar to the Norton dome were already known in the nineteenth century: I discuss four nineteenth century authors who wrote about such systems, namely Poisson, Duhamel, Boussinesq and Bertrand. However, I argue that their discussion of such systems was very different from the contemporary discussion about the Norton dome, because physicists in the nineteenth century conceived of determinism in essentially different ways: whereas in the contemporary literature on determinism in classical physics, determinism is usually taken to be a property of the equations of physics, in the nineteenth century determinism was primarily taken to be a presupposition of theories in physics, and as such it was not necessarily affected by the possible existence of systems such as the Norton dome. 相似文献
218.
Katrina van Wieringen Robert A. Cribbie 《The British journal of mathematical and statistical psychology》2014,67(2):213-230
The purpose of this study was to evaluate a modified test of equivalence for conducting normative comparisons when distribution shapes are non‐normal and variances are unequal. A Monte Carlo study was used to compare the empirical Type I error rates and power of the proposed Schuirmann–Yuen test of equivalence, which utilizes trimmed means, with that of the previously recommended Schuirmann and Schuirmann–Welch tests of equivalence when the assumptions of normality and variance homogeneity are satisfied, as well as when they are not satisfied. The empirical Type I error rates of the Schuirmann–Yuen were much closer to the nominal α level than those of the Schuirmann or Schuirmann–Welch tests, and the power of the Schuirmann–Yuen was substantially greater than that of the Schuirmann or Schuirmann–Welch tests when distributions were skewed or outliers were present. The Schuirmann–Yuen test is recommended for assessing clinical significance with normative comparisons. 相似文献
219.
In several recent studies, saccadic eye movements were found to be influenced by concurrent reaching movements. The authors investigated whether that influence originates in limb kinematic or kinetic signals. To dissociate those 2 possibilities, the authors required participants (N = 6) to generate pointing movements with a mass that either resisted or assisted limb motion. With practice, participants were able to generate pointing responses with very similar kinematics but whose kinetics varied in a systematic manner. The results showed that saccadic output was altered by the amount of force required to move the arm, consistent with an influence from limb kinetic signals. Because the interaction occurred before the pointing response began, the authors conclude that a predictive signal related to limb kinetics modulates saccadic output during tasks requiring eye-hand coordination. 相似文献
220.
Ridderinkhof KR van den Wildenberg WP Segalowitz SJ Carter CS 《Brain and cognition》2004,56(2):129-140
Convergent evidence highlights the differential contributions of various regions of the prefrontal cortex in the service of cognitive control, but little is understood about how the brain determines and communicates the need to recruit cognitive control, and how such signals instigate the implementation of appropriate performance adjustments. Here we review recent progress from cognitive neuroscience in examining some of the main constituent processes of cognitive control as involved in dynamic decision making: goal-directed action selection, response activation and inhibition, performance monitoring, and reward-based learning. Medial frontal cortex is found to be involved in performance monitoring: evaluating outcome vis-a-vis expectancy, and detecting performance errors or conflicting response tendencies. Lateral and orbitofrontal divisions of prefrontal cortex are involved in subsequently implementing appropriate adjustments. 相似文献