首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   1473篇
  免费   113篇
  2023年   16篇
  2022年   15篇
  2021年   28篇
  2020年   56篇
  2019年   43篇
  2018年   69篇
  2017年   60篇
  2016年   72篇
  2015年   62篇
  2014年   48篇
  2013年   233篇
  2012年   75篇
  2011年   87篇
  2010年   61篇
  2009年   58篇
  2008年   95篇
  2007年   71篇
  2006年   52篇
  2005年   43篇
  2004年   32篇
  2003年   46篇
  2002年   41篇
  2001年   42篇
  2000年   25篇
  1999年   18篇
  1998年   11篇
  1997年   10篇
  1996年   13篇
  1995年   6篇
  1994年   4篇
  1993年   5篇
  1991年   6篇
  1990年   5篇
  1989年   4篇
  1988年   3篇
  1986年   3篇
  1985年   5篇
  1984年   3篇
  1983年   6篇
  1982年   7篇
  1980年   3篇
  1978年   4篇
  1976年   2篇
  1975年   6篇
  1974年   2篇
  1973年   3篇
  1972年   2篇
  1971年   3篇
  1938年   3篇
  1937年   3篇
排序方式: 共有1586条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
991.
992.
993.
Many cognitive abilities, including working memory and reasoning ability, decline with progressing age. In this study, we investigated whether four weeks of intensive working memory training would enhance working memory and reasoning performance in an age-comparative setting. Groups of 34 young (19–36 years) and 27 older (62–77 years) adults practiced tasks representing the three functional categories in the facet model of working memory capacity: storage and processing, relational integration, and supervision. The data were compared to those of a young and an old active control group who practiced tasks with low working memory demands. A cognitive test battery measuring near and far transfer was administered before and after training. Both age groups showed increased working memory performance in the trained tasks and in one structurally similar, but nontrained, task. Young adults also improved in a task measuring word-position binding in working memory. However, we found no far transfer to reasoning in either age group. The results provide evidence that working memory performance can be improved throughout the life span. However, in contrast to a previous study in which each facet of working memory capacity was trained separately, the present study showed that training multiple functional categories simultaneously induces less transfer.  相似文献   
994.
Freud's concept of the death instinct has given rise to many different interpretations which have often been contradictory. It is in fact already possible to discern two diametrically opposite meanings of this concept in Freud's work from 1920—Beyond the Pleasure Principle—in which he first introduced the concept of the death instinct. In this paper, it is argued that both these meanings are relevant in describing psychical life, although only one of these meanings actually qualifies for the concept “death instinct”. Beyond the Pleasure Principle was written in order to try to understand some everyday, as well as clinical phenomena which could not be explained by the so-called pleasure principle. Freud postulated something beyond the pleasure principle, which initially seemed to have to do with binding energy. I will preserve this idea and attempt to develop it within the context of a phenomenological analysis of time. The temporalization of the subject involves a very basic affirmation of existence, in that the subject experiences something constant, something that can be said to possess the quality of a gestalt. I propose that that which is beyond the pleasure principle—this binding of energy—should be understood as the opposite of the idea of a primordial death instinct striving towards death. In this case, that which is beyond the pleasure principle reflects an original affirmation of existence, which could be said to correspond to Freud's first meaning of the death instinct. The second meaning—for which the name “death instinct” seems to be applicable—concerns the discharge of energy, which from a temporal point of view shows itself as a tendency to dissolution. The concept of the death instinct in its various meanings is discussed in connection with phenomenological reflections on time, which is a different approach from Freud's attempt to ground the death instinct in biology.  相似文献   
995.
Book Review     
LOGIC AND ONTOLOGY

PETER SIMONS,Parts: a study in ontology.Oxford , Clarendon Press. 1987. Xiii + 390 PP. £40.

GERHARD HEYER, Generische Kennzeichnungen. Zur Logik und Ontologie generischer Bedeutung. Miinchen und Wien: Philosophia Verlag, 1987. 289pp. DM 138.

GÖDEL STUDIES

GÖDEL , O. TAuSSKY-TODD, S. C. KLEEnE and g. kreisel. Gödel remembered: Salzburg 10-12 July 1983. Edited and foreword by P. Weingartner and L. Schmetterer. Naples: Bibliopolis, 1987. 186 pp, 10 plts. $49.95.

S. G. SHANKER (ed.), Gödel's theorem in focus. London: Croom Helm, 1988. 261 + ix pp. £25.00 (hardback)/£l1.95 (paperback).

SET THEORY AND NUMBER

ALFRED TARSKI and STEVEN GIVANT, A formulization of set theory without variables. (American Mathematical Society Colloquium Publications, volume 41.) Providence,Rhnde Is!and: American Mathematical Society. 1987. 280 pp. $60.00.

J. Bidelow, Thr reality of numbers. A physicalist 's philosophy of mathematics. Oxford: Clarrndon Press. 1988. viii+ 193 pp. £25.

MISCELLANEOUS

PETER J. STEINBERGER, Logic and politics. Hegel's philosophy of' right. New Haven and London: Yale University Press, 1988. xiii + 254 pp. £22.00/$29.00.

MICHELE MALATESTA, La logical primaria. Strumenti per un dialogo tra le Due Culture Napoli and Roma: LER, 1988.193 pp. 25000 lire.  相似文献   
996.
Moderated mediation effects on attitude toward environmental policy measures of a self‐enhancement vs. self‐transcendence value orientation were examined in two studies. Study 1 (n = 91) showed that for policy measures incurring nontransparent personal costs, influence of value orientation on attitude is fully mediated by environmental concern, whereas for measures incurring transparent personal costs, influence is partially mediated by environmental concern. Study 2 (n = 71) showed that while influence of value orientation on attitude toward eco‐labeled electricity is not mediated by environmental concern, it is fully mediated by reluctance to pay surcharges for eco‐labeled electricity. Value orientation was also shown to moderate influence of reluctance to pay on attitude in that reluctance had an effect only for a self‐enhancement value orientation.  相似文献   
997.
Academically and vocationally educated parents were asked to assess their child's academic competencies and motivation when the child was in preschool, then every 2 years till the end of the child's comprehensive school. It was found that the initial increasing trend in the parental attributions of competence stabilized and even turned downwards and that the education‐ and gender‐bound differences in the attributions, already manifest at the preschool stage, were still in evidence and had got more varied forms. It is suggested that parental perceptions of competencies are activated in a socially structured field of meanings and that highly educated parents construe their educational reality in terms of the social representation of natural giftedness.  相似文献   
998.
999.
Nineteen upper-middle class, pre-marital, college couples volunteered to participate in a study on interpersonal relationships. Couples received a modified Rorschach, thematic test, Marriage Value Inventory, a sex questionnaire, and an intensive depth interview. A Q sort of 100 personality items was used to assess the Ss. It was hypothesized that couples would show smaller discrepancies than randomized pairs, except for items relating to dominance, dependency, nurturance, exhibitionism, masculinity and femininity, where the reverse was predicted. Results confirmed the homogamy prediction, but only the dependency need was significant of the complementary predictions. Perception scores and expressive behavior are seen as potential adjuncts to questionnaires and inventories in predicting marital choice.  相似文献   
1000.
This study examined the efficacy of family constellation seminars (FCSs) on individuals' experience in their personal social systems, especially the experience of belonging, autonomy, accord, and confidence. We conducted a single‐blind, stratified and balanced, randomized controlled trial. Participants were 208 adults (M = 48 years, SD = 10, 79% women) who were randomly allocated either to the intervention group (3‐day FCSs; 64 active participants, 40 observing participants) or to the wait‐list group (64 active participants, 40 observing participants). Change was measured short‐term (2‐week and 4‐month follow‐up) using the Experience In Social Systems Questionnaire, personal domain (EXIS.pers). EXIS.pers is a new outcome measure being applied for the first time in evaluation research. In addition, we used interpersonal scales derived from established measures (Outcome Questionnaire, OQ‐45; Tool for the Evaluation of the Psychotherapeutic Progress, FEP). The average person in the intervention group showed improved experience in personal social systems, as compared with approximately 73% of the wait‐list group after 2 weeks (total score: Cohen's d = .61, p = .000) and 69% of the wait‐list group after 4 months (total score: d = .53, p = .000). The results were confirmed in per‐protocol analyses (n = 191) by the results of the EXIS.pers dimensions (Belonging, Autonomy, Accord, and Confidence) and the interpersonal scales derived from the OQ‐45 and FEP. No adverse events were reported. This RCT provides first evidence that FCSs tend to positively influence participants' experience in their social systems.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号