全文获取类型
收费全文 | 696篇 |
免费 | 25篇 |
专业分类
721篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 4篇 |
2023年 | 6篇 |
2022年 | 3篇 |
2021年 | 8篇 |
2020年 | 9篇 |
2019年 | 15篇 |
2018年 | 19篇 |
2017年 | 21篇 |
2016年 | 25篇 |
2015年 | 26篇 |
2014年 | 23篇 |
2013年 | 58篇 |
2012年 | 33篇 |
2011年 | 47篇 |
2010年 | 23篇 |
2009年 | 29篇 |
2008年 | 49篇 |
2007年 | 35篇 |
2006年 | 30篇 |
2005年 | 27篇 |
2004年 | 29篇 |
2003年 | 25篇 |
2002年 | 27篇 |
2001年 | 14篇 |
2000年 | 9篇 |
1999年 | 8篇 |
1998年 | 10篇 |
1996年 | 9篇 |
1995年 | 4篇 |
1994年 | 4篇 |
1993年 | 3篇 |
1992年 | 3篇 |
1991年 | 5篇 |
1990年 | 4篇 |
1989年 | 5篇 |
1988年 | 5篇 |
1987年 | 4篇 |
1986年 | 5篇 |
1985年 | 3篇 |
1984年 | 5篇 |
1982年 | 5篇 |
1981年 | 3篇 |
1980年 | 2篇 |
1978年 | 5篇 |
1977年 | 2篇 |
1976年 | 5篇 |
1974年 | 4篇 |
1973年 | 3篇 |
1971年 | 3篇 |
1968年 | 5篇 |
排序方式: 共有721条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
281.
James A. Keller 《International Journal for Philosophy of Religion》2008,64(3):179-180
282.
283.
284.
Heidi I. Brummert Lennings Wayne A. Warburton 《Journal of experimental social psychology》2011,47(4):794-799
Five decades of research have shown clear links between exposure to violent visual media and subsequent aggression, however there has been little research that directly compares the effects of exposure to violent visual versus auditory media, or which has experimentally tested the effect of violent song lyrics with musical ‘tone’ held constant. In the current study 194 participants heard music either with or without lyrics, and with or without a violent music video, and were then given the chance to aggress using the hot sauce paradigm. Musical tone was held constant across groups, and a fifth (control) group had no media exposure at all. Experimental groups, on average, were significantly more aggressive than controls. The strongest effect was elicited by exposure to violent lyrics, regardless of whether violent imagery accompanied the music, and regardless of various person-based characteristics. Implications for theories of media violence and models of aggression are discussed. 相似文献
285.
Heidi N. KeiserScott R. Ross 《Personality and individual differences》2011,51(1):39-44
Few empirical studies have investigated the relationship between Gray’s Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory (RST) and the Five-Factor Model (FFM) of personality. In a large sample of undergraduates (N = 779), we examined the relationship between FFM domains and facets and the revised RST (see Gray & McNaughton, 2000). Regression and partial correlation analyses indicated that only FFM Agreeableness discriminates between the BIS and FFFS. Other differences at the facet level were found for Neuroticism facets of Self-Consciousness and Angry Hostility (negatively), Agreeableness facets of Compliance and Modesty, and Conscientiousness facets of Self-Discipline and Deliberation. These findings emphasize social inhibition and constraint in the BIS, compared to the FFFS. 相似文献
286.
Smith RH Webster JM Parrott WG Eyre HL 《Journal of personality and social psychology》2002,83(1):138-159
Although scholarly traditions assume that shame results more from the public exposure of a transgression or incompetence than guilt does, this distinction has little empirical support. Four studies, using either undergraduate participants' responses to hypothetical scenarios, their remembered experiences, or the coding of literary passages, reexamined this issue. Supporting traditional claims, public exposure of both moral (transgressions) and nonmoral (incompetence) experiences was associated more with shame than with guilt. Shame was also more strongly linked with nonmoral experiences of inferiority, suggesting 2 core features of shame: its links with public exposure and with negative self-evaluation. The distinctive features of guilt included remorse, self-blame, and the private feelings associated with a troubled conscience. 相似文献
287.
Improved picture naming in chronic aphasia after TMS to part of right Broca's area: an open-protocol study 总被引:5,自引:0,他引:5
Naeser MA Martin PI Nicholas M Baker EH Seekins H Kobayashi M Theoret H Fregni F Maria-Tormos J Kurland J Doron KW Pascual-Leone A 《Brain and language》2005,93(1):95-105
Functional imaging studies with nonfluent aphasia patients have observed "over-activation" in right (R) language homologues. This may represent a maladaptive strategy; suppression may result in language improvement. We applied slow, 1 Hz repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) to an anterior portion of R Broca's homologue daily, for 10 days in four aphasia patients who were 5-11 years poststroke. Significant improvement was observed in picture naming at 2 months post-rTMS, with lasting benefit at 8 months in three patients. This preliminary, open trial suggests that rTMS may provide a novel treatment approach for aphasia by possibly modulating the distributed, bi-hemispheric language network. 相似文献
288.
The background page on which a stimulus is presented can influence the allocation of attention to that stimulus. The purpose of this study was to learn if there are hemispheric asymmetries in how background distraction affects attentional processing. Asymmetries were investigated by having right eye dominant subjects perform line bisections and manipulating the side of background distraction (right versus left), the eye of regard (right versus left), and the type of attention allocated (focal versus global). Overall subjects bisected lines to the left of center (pseudoneglect) and when viewing with the right eye (versus left) deviated more to the left. Subjects had more background distraction when viewing symbol than solid lines. Although overall, bias did not differ with the side of background distraction or the line being on one side or the other, when subjects viewed symbol, but not solid lines, this leftward bias was increased when the line was displaced to the right, thereby increasing the size of the left sided background. These findings suggest that when engaging the left hemisphere by using focused attention and placing the line on the right side, there is more distraction than when the right hemisphere is engaged. 相似文献
289.
James A. Keller 《International Journal for Philosophy of Religion》2002,51(3):223-225
Publications Received
Books received 相似文献290.
Catrin Griffiths Heidi Williamson Fabio Zucchelli Nicole Paraskeva Tim Moss 《Journal of Contemporary Psychotherapy》2018,48(4):189-204
Body image dissatisfaction (BID) and weight self-stigma are prevalent and associated with physical and psychological ill-health. Acceptance and Commitment Therapy (ACT) is increasingly employed for both, yet little is known about its effectiveness. Searches of 12 databases identified six studies using online, face-to-face or self-help ACT interventions for BID or weight self-stigma, of varying duration and intensity. Their effectiveness and quality were evaluated. Two reported improved BID, three improved weight self-stigma, and one reported no impact on weight self-stigma. Methodological issues (small sample sizes, lack of allocation concealment, attention control and long-term follow up) impacted the validity of findings. Due to the small number of studies and poor study quality, the effectiveness of ACT for BID and weight self-stigma remains unclear. Nonetheless findings suggest psychological flexibility may facilitate reduction in BID and weight self-stigma and indicate that brief online as well as lengthy face-to-face delivery may be useful. Suggestions for further research are made. 相似文献