首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   170154篇
  免费   7752篇
  国内免费   485篇
  2021年   1561篇
  2020年   2830篇
  2019年   3460篇
  2018年   3492篇
  2017年   3949篇
  2016年   4618篇
  2015年   3948篇
  2014年   4831篇
  2013年   23485篇
  2012年   4447篇
  2011年   3498篇
  2010年   3881篇
  2009年   4794篇
  2008年   3754篇
  2007年   3309篇
  2006年   3980篇
  2005年   3943篇
  2004年   3407篇
  2003年   3013篇
  2002年   2851篇
  2001年   3118篇
  2000年   3008篇
  1999年   3047篇
  1998年   2832篇
  1997年   2652篇
  1996年   2564篇
  1995年   2404篇
  1994年   2382篇
  1993年   2313篇
  1992年   2530篇
  1991年   2345篇
  1990年   2222篇
  1989年   2164篇
  1988年   2120篇
  1987年   2142篇
  1986年   2133篇
  1985年   2342篇
  1984年   2476篇
  1983年   2257篇
  1982年   2369篇
  1981年   2325篇
  1980年   2158篇
  1979年   2158篇
  1978年   2153篇
  1977年   2130篇
  1976年   1904篇
  1975年   1965篇
  1974年   2033篇
  1973年   1865篇
  1972年   1494篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
181.
Human communication is thoroughly context bound. We present two experiments investigating the importance of the shared context, that is, the amount of knowledge two interlocutors have in common, for the successful emergence and use of novel conventions. Using a referential communication task where black‐and‐white pictorial symbols are used to convey colors, pairs of participants build shared conventions peculiar to their dyad without experimenter feedback, relying purely on ostensive‐inferential communication. Both experiments demonstrate that access to the visual context promotes more successful communication. Importantly, success improves cumulatively, supporting the view that pairs establish conventional ways of using the symbols to communicate. Furthermore, Experiment 2 suggests that dyads with access to the visual context successfully adapt the conventions built for one color space to another color space, unlike dyads lacking it. In linking experimental pragmatics with language evolution, the study illustrates the benefits of exploring the emergence of linguistic conventions using an ostensive‐inferential model of communication.  相似文献   
182.
183.
Do the conditions under which promises are made determine whether they ought to be kept? Philosophers have placed a number of conditions on promising which, they hold, must be met in order to make promise-keeping obligatory. In so doing, they have distinguished valid promises from invalid promises and justified promises from promises that are not justified. Considering such conditions, one by one, we argue that they are mistaken. In the first place, the conditions they lay down are not necessary for either valid or justified promise-making. In the second place, promises need not meet such conditions in order to create moral obligations. In general, such analyses of promising fail because they suffer from a confusion between promise-making and promise-keeping. Philosophers have wrongly supposed that obligations to keep promises are dependent upon, or derivable from, the quality of the promises themselves, at the time they are made, instead of focusing on conditions that must be satisfied at the time when promises are supposed to be kept. It is not the quality of a promise that determines an obligation to keep it but the rightness or wrongness of performing the promised act.  相似文献   
184.
Ohne ZusammenfassungIn dankbarer Erinnerung an meinen verehrten Münchener Lehrer Kurt Huber, dessen frühzeitigen gewaltsamen Tod (13. 7. 43) wir noch immer beklagen. Die Arbeit entsprang einer seiner letzten wissenschaftlichen Anregungen.  相似文献   
185.
186.
Wray  K. Brad 《Synthese》2019,196(3):819-832
Synthese - Hoyningen-Huene (Systematicity: the nature of science, Oxford University Press, Oxford, 2013) develops an account of what science is, distinguishing it from common sense. According to...  相似文献   
187.
188.
189.
190.
Are different responses differentially associable with their consequences? An overshadowing design was used to try to answer this question for three responses previously studied in golden hamsters. In Experiment I, scrabbling was rapidly suppressed by electric shock punishment, and it overshadowed a tone which occurred between scrabbling and shock. In Experiment II, no evidence of response-shock association was obtained when open rearing was the punished response, and open rearing did not overshadow the tone. Punishment had some effect on face washing, but there was no statistically significant overshadowing with this response. These results are consistent with the idea that differences in punishment suppression among these three responses have an associative basis. They also demonstrate the usefulness of a novel paradigm for studying “preparedness” of response-reinforcer associations.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号