首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   1425篇
  免费   88篇
  2023年   13篇
  2022年   7篇
  2021年   14篇
  2020年   30篇
  2019年   44篇
  2018年   57篇
  2017年   53篇
  2016年   66篇
  2015年   39篇
  2014年   52篇
  2013年   195篇
  2012年   88篇
  2011年   89篇
  2010年   44篇
  2009年   54篇
  2008年   68篇
  2007年   45篇
  2006年   68篇
  2005年   45篇
  2004年   71篇
  2003年   51篇
  2002年   53篇
  2001年   19篇
  2000年   22篇
  1999年   18篇
  1998年   22篇
  1997年   17篇
  1996年   6篇
  1995年   17篇
  1994年   5篇
  1993年   8篇
  1992年   8篇
  1991年   9篇
  1990年   8篇
  1989年   7篇
  1988年   8篇
  1987年   9篇
  1986年   5篇
  1985年   5篇
  1984年   7篇
  1982年   4篇
  1980年   6篇
  1979年   4篇
  1978年   6篇
  1977年   4篇
  1975年   4篇
  1974年   5篇
  1971年   5篇
  1967年   3篇
  1965年   3篇
排序方式: 共有1513条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
861.
862.
863.
Editorial     
  相似文献   
864.
Drawing on a communications model of persuasion ( Hovland, Janis, & Kelley, 1953 ), this study examined the effect of target appearance on feminists' and nonfeminists' perceptions of a speaker delivering a feminist or an antifeminist message. One hundred three college women watched one of four videotaped speeches that varied by content (profeminist vs. antifeminist) and target appearance ("feminine" vs. "masculine"). Self-identified feminists responded more favorably to a profeminist message when it was presented by a feminine than a masculine-appearing speaker and expressed less feminist attitudes after viewing a masculine-appearing feminist speaker. Nonfeminists' evaluations did not vary as a function of speaker appearance or message content. Implications for the communication of feminist ideology are discussed.  相似文献   
865.
866.
The impact of event outcome and prior belief on scientific reasoning was investigated within a real‐world oral health context. Participants (N= 144; ranging from 3 to 11 years) were given hypothesis‐testing tasks and asked to explain their answers. Participants were presented with information that was either consistent or inconsistent with their own beliefs. Each task consisted of scenarios in which the outcome was either good or bad oral health. When the information was belief consistent and the outcome was good, or when the information was belief inconsistent and the outcome was bad, children were more likely to choose scientifically appropriate tests of the stated hypothesis (i.e. manipulate only one variable). Evidence‐based explanations were associated with scientifically appropriate choices in the good‐outcome, belief‐inconsistent scenario and the belief‐consistent, bad‐outcome scenario. Participants' performance on these tasks is explained by considering the plausibility of causal variables. A control of variables strategy was used to test hypotheses in cases in which the evidence was consistent with participants' beliefs and knowledge of causal mechanisms. In contrast, when the evidence was inconsistent with participants' beliefs, children chose to manipulate behaviours likely to lead to a positive health outcome. These findings demonstrate that context and prior knowledge interact to play an important role in children's scientific reasoning.  相似文献   
867.
868.
869.
Students' low confidence, particularly in numerical topics, is thought to be a barrier to keeping them engaged with education. We studied the effects on confidence of exposure to a peer's work of varying quality (very good or bad) and neatness (messy or neat). Previous research underpinned our hypothesis that a peer's bad-quality work—which students rarely see—might boost student confidence more than very good work. We also predicted that a peer's very good work—which students are often shown—might be less discouraging if it were messy, suggesting it required effort and struggle. However, in experiments with university students and low-educated adults, these hypotheses were not supported, and all participants decreased in confidence after seeing any peer work. The failure to find support for these hypotheses can inform future research into social comparison effects on self-confidence in numerical topics. These results also have practical implications for teachers and managers who are expected to provide examples of peer work.  相似文献   
870.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号