首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   241篇
  免费   7篇
  2022年   2篇
  2021年   3篇
  2020年   5篇
  2019年   8篇
  2018年   10篇
  2017年   7篇
  2016年   7篇
  2015年   4篇
  2014年   5篇
  2013年   46篇
  2012年   13篇
  2011年   5篇
  2010年   7篇
  2009年   3篇
  2008年   17篇
  2007年   8篇
  2006年   8篇
  2005年   5篇
  2004年   10篇
  2003年   5篇
  2002年   5篇
  2001年   7篇
  2000年   2篇
  1998年   3篇
  1997年   2篇
  1996年   2篇
  1995年   2篇
  1994年   5篇
  1992年   2篇
  1991年   5篇
  1989年   2篇
  1988年   2篇
  1987年   2篇
  1985年   3篇
  1983年   1篇
  1982年   2篇
  1981年   1篇
  1978年   1篇
  1974年   2篇
  1973年   2篇
  1971年   2篇
  1970年   1篇
  1969年   2篇
  1968年   1篇
  1967年   1篇
  1965年   1篇
  1960年   1篇
  1959年   1篇
  1958年   2篇
  1957年   2篇
排序方式: 共有248条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
171.
How automatic are social judgments?   总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2  
Do people infer personality dispositions automatically when they encode behavior? Tulving's encoding-specificity paradigm was adapted to test three operational indicants of automatism: absence of intention, of interference from other mental activity, and of awareness. Recruited for a digit-recall study, subjects read sentences describing actions during the retention interval of either an easy or a difficult digit recall task. Later, sentence recall was cued by (a) disposition cues, (b) strong semantic associates to the sentence actor, or (c) words representing the gist of the sentence, or (d) sentence recall was not cued. Awareness was measured immediately after the last sentence was read. Disposition-cued recall was higher than (b) or (d) and was unaffected by digit recall difficulty. Awareness of making dispositional inferences was only weakly correlated with disposition-cued recall. Results suggest that disposition inferences occurred at encoding, without intention, without interference by differential drain on processing capacity, and with little awareness. Thus, making dispositional inferences seems to be largely, but not entirely, automatic.  相似文献   
172.
A content analysis is reported of child-rearing manuals published in sixteenth to nineteenth centuries and of the themes of the Don Juan plays in seventeenth to twentieth centuries. The relationship between the content of the manuals and the themes of the plays supports the thesis that child-rearing patterns advocated in one century are reflected in the next in the attitudes expressed in popular cultural activities towards aggression, achievement and sex roles.  相似文献   
173.
174.
An inexpensive sound-activated switch suitable for experiments measuring vocal-response times is described. This switch is comparable to many commercially produced switches in terms of its control over input sensitivity and response delay times, but it can be assembled for a fraction of the cost. Additionally, the present switch provides many features not found on standard commercial models such as a built-in speaker for monitoring the input signal and output jacks to facilitate simultaneous recording of this signal. Reliability estimates suggest that performance for this switch is comparable to that of a Scientific Prototype Model 761-G when both units are equated for frequency sensitivity and response delay time. In all, this switch offers a reliable and practical alternative to commercially produced devices for experiments in which the onset of an auditory signal must be accurately detected.  相似文献   
175.
A system for creating and running psychological experiments on IBM PC compatible computers is described. The system attempts to reconcile the demand for flexible and powerful experimental software with the need for systems that are easy to understand and modify. Experiments are created by building a series of displays with variable compositions, durations, and stimulus onset asynchronies. The number, type, location, color, and perceptibility of stimuli within these displays can be systematically varied. Reaction time and other response measures can be collected from the keyboard, a voice-activated switch, or other external switches. The system is assembled from a series of Turbo Pascal and Turbo C routines that may be modified by the user or incorporated into new routines. Custom modules can be added easily to any of the system’s menus. The system is best suited for discrete trial experiments with target detection, perceptual priming, lexical decision, or recognition memory tasks.  相似文献   
176.
    
  相似文献   
177.
178.
There is a recent increase in interest of Bayesian analysis. However, little effort has been made thus far to directly incorporate background knowledge via the prior distribution into the analyses. This process might be especially useful in the context of latent growth mixture modeling when one or more of the latent groups are expected to be relatively small due to what we refer to as limited data. We argue that the use of Bayesian statistics has great advantages in limited data situations, but only if background knowledge can be incorporated into the analysis via prior distributions. We highlight these advantages through a data set including patients with burn injuries and analyze trajectories of posttraumatic stress symptoms using the Bayesian framework following the steps of the WAMBS-checklist. In the included example, we illustrate how to obtain background information using previous literature based on a systematic literature search and by using expert knowledge. Finally, we show how to translate this knowledge into prior distributions and we illustrate the importance of conducting a prior sensitivity analysis. Although our example is from the trauma field, the techniques we illustrate can be applied to any field.  相似文献   
179.
A novel paradigm was developed to study the behavior of groups of networked people searching a problem space. The authors examined how different network structures affect the propagation of information in laboratory-created groups. Participants made numerical guesses and received scores that were also made available to their neighbors in the network. The networks were compared on speed of discovery and convergence on the optimal solution. One experiment showed that individuals within a group tend to converge on similar solutions even when there is an equally valid alternative solution. Two additional studies demonstrated that the optimal network structure depends on the problem space being explored, with networks that incorporate spatially based cliques having an advantage for problems that benefit from broad exploration, and networks with greater long-range connectivity having an advantage for problems requiring less exploration. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2008 APA, all rights reserved).  相似文献   
180.
Hill and Willoughby’s (Sex Roles, 53:531–544, 2005) Genderism and Transphobia Scale (GTS), originally developed in Canada, was examined with a Hong Kong sample. Undergraduate students, 82 female and 121 male (total n?=?203), completed a Chinese version of the instrument. Overall scores and factor structure of the Hong Kong sample were compared with Hill and Willoughby’s Canadian data. Gender differences in transphobia were investigated, both in terms of the participants’ gender as well as the gender of the gender variant persons to whom GTS items referred. Transphobia was higher in Hong Kong than in Canada. The factor structure for Hong Kong differed from Canada. Five factors were identified (with a gender effect on Factors II and V). They were: I, Anti Sissy Prejudice; II, Anti Trans Violence; III, Trans Unnaturalness; IV, Trans Immorality; and V, Background Genderism. Hong Kong men were more transphobic than women. Gender variance in men was viewed less favourably than in women.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号