首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   64468篇
  免费   2635篇
  国内免费   37篇
  67140篇
  2020年   711篇
  2019年   879篇
  2018年   1270篇
  2017年   1244篇
  2016年   1328篇
  2015年   914篇
  2014年   1125篇
  2013年   5414篇
  2012年   2033篇
  2011年   2122篇
  2010年   1297篇
  2009年   1292篇
  2008年   1942篇
  2007年   1932篇
  2006年   1710篇
  2005年   1578篇
  2004年   1512篇
  2003年   1362篇
  2002年   1438篇
  2001年   2226篇
  2000年   2122篇
  1999年   1587篇
  1998年   799篇
  1997年   696篇
  1996年   648篇
  1995年   620篇
  1994年   596篇
  1993年   618篇
  1992年   1288篇
  1991年   1216篇
  1990年   1205篇
  1989年   1082篇
  1988年   1030篇
  1987年   1015篇
  1986年   998篇
  1985年   1058篇
  1984年   863篇
  1983年   744篇
  1982年   601篇
  1979年   835篇
  1978年   632篇
  1975年   749篇
  1974年   823篇
  1973年   797篇
  1972年   699篇
  1971年   635篇
  1969年   589篇
  1968年   728篇
  1967年   703篇
  1966年   617篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
191.
Effects of alternative reinforcement sources: A reevaluation   总被引:3,自引:3,他引:0       下载免费PDF全文
The effects of two alternative sources of food delivery on the key-peck responding of pigeons were examined. Pecking was maintained by a variable-interval 3-min schedule. In the presence of this schedule in different conditions, either a variable-time 3-min schedule delivering food independently of responding or an equivalent schedule that required a minimum 2-s pause between a key peck and food delivery (a differential-reinforcement-of-other-behavior schedule) was added. The differential-reinforcement-of-other-behavior schedule reduced response rates more than did the variable-time schedule in most instances. The delay between a key peck and the next reinforcer consistently was longer under the differential-reinforcement-of-other-behavior schedule than under the variable-time schedule. Response rates and median delay between responses and reinforcers were negatively correlated. These results contradict earlier conclusions about the behavioral effects of alternative reinforcement. They suggest that an interpretation in terms of response–reinforcer contiguity is consistent with the data.  相似文献   
192.
193.
194.
195.
196.
Human motor learning is often measured by error scores. The convention of using mean absolute error, mean constant error, and variable error shows lack of desirable parsimony and interpretability. This paper provides the background of error measurement and states criticisms of conventional methodology. A parsimonious model of error analysis is provided, along with operationalized interpretations and implications for motor learning. Teaching, interpreting, and using error scores in research may be simplified and facilitated with the model.  相似文献   
197.
Cutoff scores are used in a variety of settings to help managers make personnel decisions. Although cutoff scores are used widely, information about their derivation and appropriate use is scattered in the legal, psychometric, and professional literatures. The purpose of this paper is to analyze critically and to integrate these diverse literatures, to summarize what is known and what is unknown about the use and misuse of cutoff scores. Alternative methods are examined in relation to legal precedents set by the courts, to psychometric principles, and to the various sets of professional guidelines. Directions for future research are suggested, and we conclude with a set of recommendations regarding acceptable professional practice in this area.  相似文献   
198.
Individuals with stronger sex-role identities may be expected to show greater conformity to sex-typed behaviors. Proceeding from this assumption, the sex-role status and sex of students using different styles of carrying books were compared. This study also examined whether over-all hand preference or age were related to carrying styles or sex-role status. Subjects completed a questionnaire comprised of the 24-item Personal Attributes Questionnaire of Spence and Helmreich, a 6-item scale of hand preference and a self-rating of sex-role identity. Afterwards, each subject's carrying style was covertly observed and recorded. Most subjects carried their materials in a sex-typical way or in book bags (for which no sex-typed carrying styles were discovered). As in previous studies, men in this study scored higher than women on masculinity and androgyny and lower on femininity. Hand preference was not related to sex-role status. Carrying style was not significantly correlated with any of the predictor variables for men, perhaps because almost no variance in carrying style was observed among them. Women who used masculine carrying styles tended to be older, have greater instrumentality, and a more masculine identity.  相似文献   
199.
200.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号