全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1409篇 |
免费 | 18篇 |
专业分类
1427篇 |
出版年
2021年 | 10篇 |
2020年 | 18篇 |
2019年 | 19篇 |
2018年 | 33篇 |
2017年 | 30篇 |
2016年 | 40篇 |
2015年 | 27篇 |
2014年 | 39篇 |
2013年 | 153篇 |
2012年 | 58篇 |
2011年 | 51篇 |
2010年 | 39篇 |
2009年 | 53篇 |
2008年 | 50篇 |
2007年 | 43篇 |
2006年 | 38篇 |
2005年 | 25篇 |
2004年 | 43篇 |
2003年 | 31篇 |
2002年 | 27篇 |
2001年 | 17篇 |
2000年 | 17篇 |
1999年 | 20篇 |
1998年 | 21篇 |
1997年 | 14篇 |
1996年 | 22篇 |
1995年 | 18篇 |
1993年 | 12篇 |
1992年 | 16篇 |
1991年 | 17篇 |
1990年 | 14篇 |
1989年 | 19篇 |
1988年 | 10篇 |
1987年 | 11篇 |
1986年 | 12篇 |
1985年 | 27篇 |
1984年 | 10篇 |
1983年 | 16篇 |
1982年 | 20篇 |
1981年 | 13篇 |
1980年 | 13篇 |
1978年 | 17篇 |
1977年 | 14篇 |
1976年 | 17篇 |
1975年 | 10篇 |
1974年 | 11篇 |
1972年 | 23篇 |
1968年 | 10篇 |
1966年 | 10篇 |
1935年 | 9篇 |
排序方式: 共有1427条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
861.
Doerthe Seifert Christine Falter Hans Strasburger Mark A. Elliott 《Consciousness and cognition》2010,19(1):144-151
Patient RP suffers a unilateral right homonymous quadrant anopia but demonstrates better than chance discrimination for stimuli presented in the blind field at temporal frequencies between 33 and 47 Hz (all significant at p < .05, binomial). Examination of her reports of visual experience during blind-field discrimination suggests a more complex picture in which experiences particular to correct discrimination are not found at low-mid-gamma frequencies, but are significantly more likely than average (76%, p < .001) at a lower frequency (22 Hz) at which blindsight is not observed. We believe that visual experience may serve to support blindsight if discrimination tasks are generally impaired at frequencies outside of the low-mid-gamma band. If this is so, although generally experienced as non-specific and unstructured light, the visual experience that accompanies discrimination performance must be based upon a neural representation which includes information on the visual features present in the stimulus. 相似文献
862.
863.
Werner Wirth Tilo Hartmann Saskia Böcking Peter Vorderer Christoph Klimmt Holger Schramm 《Media Psychology》2013,16(3):493-525
In order to bridge interdisciplinary differences in Presence research and to establish connections between Presence and “older” concepts of psychology and communication, a theoretical model of the formation of Spatial Presence is proposed. It is applicable to the exposure to different media and intended to unify the existing efforts to develop a theory of Presence. The model includes assumptions about attention allocation, mental models, and involvement, and considers the role of media factors and user characteristics as well, thus incorporating much previous work. It is argued that a commonly accepted model of Spatial Presence is the only solution to secure further progress within the international, interdisciplinary and multiple-paradigm community of Presence research. 相似文献
864.
Hans Madueme 《The American journal of bioethics : AJOB》2013,13(1):25-27
There is consensus that children have questionable decisional capacity and, therefore, in general a parent or a guardian must give permission to enroll a child in a research study. Moreover, freedom from duress and coercion, the cardinal rule in research involving adults, is even more important for children. This principle is embodied prominently in the Nuremberg Code (1947) and is embodied in various federal human research protection regulations. In a program named "SATURN" (Student Athletic Testing Using Random Notification), each school in the Oregon public-school system may implement a mandatory drug-testing program for high school student athletes. A prospective study to identify drug use among student-athletes, SATURN is designed both to evaluate the influence of random drug testing and to validate the survey data through identification of individuals who do not report drug use. The enrollment of students in the drug-testing study is a requirement for playing a school sport. In addition to the coercive nature of this study design, there were ethically questionable practices in recruitment, informed consent, and confidentiality. This article concerns the question of whether research can be conducted with high school students in conjunction with a mandatory drug-testing program, while adhering to prevailing ethical standards regarding human-subjects research and specifically the participation of children in research. 相似文献
865.
Inferential or epistemic conditional sentences represent a blueprint of someone’s reasoning process from premise to conclusion.
Declerck and Reed (2001) make a distinction between a direct and an indirect type. In the latter type the direction of reasoning goes backwards,
from the blatant falsehood of the consequent to the falsehood of the antecedent. We first present a modal reinterpretation
in terms of Argumentation Schemes of indirect inferential conditionals (IIC’s) in Declerck and Reed (2001). We furthermore argue for a distinction between epistemic-modal strong and deontic-modal weak IIC’s. In addition, we extend
the category of the indirect inferential conditionals in order to include several other deontic-modal subtypes. On the basis
of the undesirability of the consequent the hearer in these cases infers that the antecedent is also undesirable. In this
way the rhetoric-argumentative strategy of Reductio ad Absurdum is extended from the realm of deductive reasoning to that
of practical reasoning. 相似文献
866.
Astrid de Groot Hans M. Koot Frank C. Verhulst 《Journal of abnormal child psychology》1996,24(5):651-664
Exploratory factor analyses on 569 Youth Self-Reports and 1,221 Teacher's Report Forms of clinically referred Dutch children revealed six and eight factors respectively, very similar to the eight YSR and TRF cross-informant syndromes derived by Achenbach (1991c, 1991d). Mean cross-cultural correlations were .89 for YSR syndromes and .95 for TRF syndromes. In confirmatory factor analyses of the Dutch and American YSR and TRF factor models in cross-validation samples of 570 YSRs and 1,221 TRFs, goodness-of-fit indices were only slightly better for Dutch factor models. The American cross-informant Social Problems and Attention Problems syndromes had the poorest fit. The application of the eight American cross-informant syndromes to Dutch self-and teacher reports was supported.We wish to thank Dr. Thomas Achenbach for his comments and advice on this article.This research was supported by grants from the Sophia Foundation for Medical Research and from the Dutch National Program for Stimulation of Health Research. 相似文献
867.
868.
869.
870.