首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   1139篇
  免费   44篇
  1183篇
  2021年   7篇
  2020年   13篇
  2019年   20篇
  2018年   29篇
  2017年   28篇
  2016年   35篇
  2015年   23篇
  2014年   31篇
  2013年   135篇
  2012年   42篇
  2011年   52篇
  2010年   36篇
  2009年   47篇
  2008年   50篇
  2007年   45篇
  2006年   32篇
  2005年   26篇
  2004年   36篇
  2003年   30篇
  2002年   20篇
  2001年   16篇
  2000年   21篇
  1999年   15篇
  1998年   18篇
  1997年   10篇
  1996年   18篇
  1995年   12篇
  1992年   13篇
  1991年   14篇
  1990年   9篇
  1989年   13篇
  1987年   9篇
  1986年   8篇
  1985年   17篇
  1983年   14篇
  1982年   15篇
  1981年   7篇
  1980年   7篇
  1978年   9篇
  1977年   11篇
  1976年   16篇
  1975年   7篇
  1973年   8篇
  1972年   20篇
  1970年   8篇
  1969年   6篇
  1968年   6篇
  1966年   7篇
  1935年   9篇
  1931年   6篇
排序方式: 共有1183条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
991.
Subjects were instructed to order stimuli, consisting of a white and a black part, in accordance with the perceived foreground-strength of the white (or black) part. It appeared that this task can be carried out consistently. The order is explained on the basis of Structural Information Theory. It is argued that this task involves central processes, and that the strength depends on the preference for an interpretation, in which the white (black) part is a foreground on a black (white) background, with respect to the opposed interpretation in which the white (black) part is the background and the black (white) part is foreground.  相似文献   
992.
993.
994.
According to a model for form and motion perception proposed by Johansson (1964), every two-dimensional change in the proximal stimulation is projected out as a motion in depth. The amount of perceived depth motion can then be predicted from the projective relationship between the proximal change and the projected motion. This prediction was tested in a series of experiments by using squares that continuously changed their sizes as stimuli, and measuring perceived distance of motion in depth. The obtained relationship between perceived and predicted distance of motion was curvilinear for all Ss. Furthermore, the majority of the Ss underestimated the motion systematically, the remainder overestimated it. Thus, the prediction given in the model could not be verified. However, an alternative projective relation based on the assumption that a fixed proportion of the change is not projected out as a motion but perceived as a change of size agreed quite well with the data, both with distance judgments and with judgments of perceived change of size.  相似文献   
995.
Adaptation to vertical field displacements dependent on head turning about a vertical axis was demonstrated under two conditions, rapid training with 100 head movements and 1-h-long training with continuous head turning. The effect of rapid training was measured with the slant estimation method. Adaptation after the longer training was ascertained by comparing the uncertainty ranges for apparent target immobility before and after the adaptation period. Adaptation to field displacements in directions parallel to the plane of the head rotation obtained under corresponding conditions was also measured and found to be somewhat greater than adaptation to vertical field displacements. The result of work by Wallach and Frey that adaptation to field displacement in the direction with the head rotation is greater than to displacement against it was corroborated. While the previous result had, been obtained with rapid adaptation and with the slant estimation method, we confirmed it with 1-h training and by measuring the uncertainty ranges before and after the adaptation period.  相似文献   
996.
R ommetveit , R., T och , H. & S vendsen , D. Effects of contingency and contrast contexts on the cognition of words. A study of stereoscopic rivalry. Scad r. Psychol ., 1968, 9, 138–144.—Two typographically very similar words (like 'hell' and 'tell') were presented in a binocular rivalry situation, each appearing after a contrast context (e.g. 'heaven') or a contingency context (e.g. 'devil') had been presented to both eyes. Context effect was then assessed in terms of the frequency with which the context-relevant word was reported as seen. The effect of contrast compared with contingency context was weak when context and test words were presented consecutively, but strong when the context stimulus appeared above the rivalry pair on the same stereogram.  相似文献   
997.
998.
Zusammenfassung Es wurde der Einfluß der Faktoren semantische Implikation und Belebtheit des Subjekts auf die Speicherung der Verneinungsformen Subjektverneinung (SV), Prädikatsverneinung (PV) und Objektverneinung (OV) untersucht. Die Analyse der Befunde ergab, daß die Verneinungsformen SV, PV und OV unterschiedlich gut gespeichert werden und die beiden Variablen semantische Implikation und Belebtheit des Subjekts die Speicherung dieser Negationsformen differentiell beeinflussen.
Semantic factors in storing negated sentences
Summary The influence of the factors semantic implication and animateness of the subject of the sentence upon the recall of the different forms of negation: negation of the subject (SV), negation of the predicate (PV) and negation of the object (OV) was investigated. It was found that the forms of negation SV, PV and OV were differently recalled and that the two variables semantic implication and animateness of the subject showed a different effect on the recall of these three forms of negation.
  相似文献   
999.
When the normal constancy process on which the apparent immobility of the visualfield during head movements is based was strengthened by the same method that produces adaptation to abnormal conditions in the constancy of visual direction, and when this training of the normal constancy process immediately preceded experimental adaptation, the effectiveness of the latter was diminished. This result applied not only to adaptation to horizontal field displacement and to vertical field displacement during turning of the head, but also to vertical field displacement during nodding of the head, a condition to which adaptation was here demonstrated for the first time.  相似文献   
1000.
Previous recommendations to employ occurrence, nonoccurrence, and overall estimates of interobserver reliability for interval data are reviewed. A rationale for comparing obtained reliability to reliability that would result from a random-chance model is explained. Formulae and graphic functions are presented to allow for the determination of chance agreement for each of the three indices, given any obtained per cent of intervals in which a response is recorded to occur. All indices are interpretable throughout the range of possible obtained values for the per cent of intervals in which a response is recorded. The level of chance agreement simply changes with changing values. Statistical procedures that could be used to determine whether obtained reliability is significantly superior to chance reliability are reviewed. These procedures are rejected because they yield significance levels that are partly a function of sample sizes and because there are no general rules to govern acceptable significance levels depending on the sizes of samples employed.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号