全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1268篇 |
免费 | 10篇 |
专业分类
1278篇 |
出版年
2021年 | 10篇 |
2020年 | 12篇 |
2019年 | 15篇 |
2018年 | 27篇 |
2017年 | 34篇 |
2016年 | 32篇 |
2015年 | 28篇 |
2014年 | 35篇 |
2013年 | 141篇 |
2012年 | 48篇 |
2011年 | 53篇 |
2010年 | 45篇 |
2009年 | 45篇 |
2008年 | 53篇 |
2007年 | 49篇 |
2006年 | 37篇 |
2005年 | 29篇 |
2004年 | 43篇 |
2003年 | 34篇 |
2002年 | 28篇 |
2001年 | 16篇 |
2000年 | 19篇 |
1999年 | 14篇 |
1998年 | 16篇 |
1997年 | 11篇 |
1996年 | 18篇 |
1995年 | 15篇 |
1994年 | 9篇 |
1992年 | 12篇 |
1991年 | 16篇 |
1990年 | 11篇 |
1989年 | 13篇 |
1988年 | 9篇 |
1987年 | 10篇 |
1986年 | 11篇 |
1985年 | 17篇 |
1984年 | 12篇 |
1983年 | 14篇 |
1982年 | 19篇 |
1981年 | 10篇 |
1980年 | 10篇 |
1978年 | 11篇 |
1977年 | 12篇 |
1976年 | 19篇 |
1975年 | 9篇 |
1974年 | 7篇 |
1972年 | 21篇 |
1971年 | 7篇 |
1970年 | 6篇 |
1935年 | 9篇 |
排序方式: 共有1278条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
881.
We propose that Vaesen's target article (a) underestimates the role of language in humans' cognitive toolkit and thereby (b) overestimates the proposed cognitive discontinuity between chimps and humans. We provide examples of labeling, numerical computation, executive control, and the relation between language and body, concluding that language plays a crucial role in "supersizing humans' cognitive toolkit." 相似文献
882.
Ijzerman H Gallucci M Pouw WT Weiβgerber SC Van Doesum NJ Williams KD 《Acta psychologica》2012,140(3):283-288
Being ostracized or excluded, even briefly and by strangers, is painful and threatens fundamental needs. Recent work by Zhong and Leonardelli (2008) found that excluded individuals perceive the room as cooler and that they desire warmer drinks. A perspective that many rely on in embodiment is the theoretical idea that people use metaphorical associations to understand social exclusion (see Landau, Meier, & Keefer, 2010). We suggest that people feel colder because they are colder. The results strongly support the idea that more complex metaphorical understandings of social relations are scaffolded onto literal changes in bodily temperature: Being excluded in an online ball tossing game leads to lower finger temperatures (Study 1), while the negative affect typically experienced after such social exclusion is alleviated after holding a cup of warm tea (Study 2). The authors discuss further implications for the interaction between body and social relations specifically, and for basic and cognitive systems in general. 相似文献
883.
Hans F. M. Crombag 《Applied cognitive psychology》2014,28(2):285-286
884.
885.
Karolina Zwierzynska Dieter Wolke Tanya S. Lereya 《Journal of abnormal child psychology》2013,41(2):309-323
Traumatic childhood experiences have been found to predict later internalizing problems. This prospective longitudinal study investigated whether repeated and intentional harm doing by peers (peer victimization) in childhood predicts internalizing symptoms in early adolescence. 3,692 children from the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC), as well as their mothers and teachers, reported on bullying in childhood (7–10 years) and internalizing problems in early adolescence (11–14 years). Controlling for prior psychopathology, family adversity, gender and IQ, being a victim of bullying was associated with higher overall scores, as well as increased odds of scoring in the severe range (>90th percentile) for emotional and depression symptoms. Victims were also more likely to show persistent depression symptoms over a 2-year period. These associations were found independent of whether mothers, teachers or the children reported on bullying. It is concluded that peer victimization in childhood is a precursor of both short-lived and persistent internalizing symptoms, underlining the importance of environmental factors such as peer relationships in the etiology of internalizing problems. 相似文献
886.
Background. It is often claimed that psychology students’ attitudes towards research methods and statistics affect course enrolment, persistence, achievement, and course climate. However, the inter‐institutional variability has been widely neglected in the research on students’ attitudes towards research methods and statistics, but it is important for didactic purposes (heterogeneity of the student population). Aims. The paper presents a scale based on findings of the social psychology of attitudes (polar and emotion‐based concept) in conjunction with a method for capturing beginning university students’ attitudes towards research methods and statistics and identifying the proportion of students having positive attitudes at the institutional level. Sample. The study based on a re‐analysis of a nationwide survey in Germany in August 2000 of all psychology students that enrolled in fall 1999/2000 (N= 1,490) and N= 44 universities. Methods. Using multilevel latent‐class analysis (MLLCA), the aim was to group students in different student attitude types and at the same time to obtain university segments based on the incidences of the different student attitude types. Results. Four student latent clusters were found that can be ranked on a bipolar attitude dimension. Membership in a cluster was predicted by age, grade point average (GPA) on school‐leaving exam, and personality traits. In addition, two university segments were found: universities with an average proportion of students with positive attitudes and universities with a high proportion of students with positive attitudes (excellent segment). Conclusions. As psychology students make up a very heterogeneous group, the use of multiple learning activities as opposed to the classical lecture course is required. 相似文献
887.
888.
889.
Goerke M Moller J Schulz-Hardt S Napiersky U Frey D 《The Journal of applied psychology》2004,89(2):279-292
In testing for the self-serving bias in performance evaluation, the authors propose that comparing managers' counterfactual and prefactual thoughts about subordinates' performance is more conclusive than the attributional approach and also offers practical advantages. In a study with 120 managers, a 4-way interaction of subordinate performance, temporal perspective, direction, and reference confirmed the predicted pattern. Managers' thoughts about how a weak performance could have been enhanced had external references, but thoughts about how such a performance could be enhanced in the future focused on the leader. This asymmetry was only observed for weak performance. Results are discussed with regard to biases in leaders' performance evaluations and to how counter- and prefactual thoughts could be used for leadership research and practice. 相似文献
890.
Manual reaction times to visual, auditory, and tactile stimuli presented simultaneously, or with a delay, were measured to test for multisensory interaction effects in a simple detection task with redundant signals. Responses to trimodal stimulus combinations were faster than those to bimodal combinations, which in turn were faster than reactions to unimodal stimuli. Response enhancement increased with decreasing auditory and tactile stimulus intensity and was a U-shaped function of stimulus onset asynchrony. Distribution inequality tests indicated that the multisensory interaction effects were larger than predicted by separate activation models, including the difference between bimodal and trimodal response facilitation. The results are discussed with respect to previous findings in a focused attention task and are compared with multisensory integration rules observed in bimodal and trimodal superior colliculus neurons in the cat and monkey. 相似文献