全文获取类型
收费全文 | 221篇 |
免费 | 12篇 |
专业分类
233篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 3篇 |
2022年 | 2篇 |
2021年 | 2篇 |
2020年 | 4篇 |
2019年 | 13篇 |
2018年 | 6篇 |
2017年 | 14篇 |
2016年 | 9篇 |
2015年 | 11篇 |
2014年 | 13篇 |
2013年 | 15篇 |
2012年 | 11篇 |
2011年 | 19篇 |
2010年 | 5篇 |
2009年 | 8篇 |
2008年 | 7篇 |
2007年 | 12篇 |
2006年 | 6篇 |
2005年 | 6篇 |
2004年 | 6篇 |
2003年 | 2篇 |
2002年 | 2篇 |
2001年 | 7篇 |
2000年 | 2篇 |
1998年 | 3篇 |
1997年 | 3篇 |
1991年 | 1篇 |
1990年 | 2篇 |
1987年 | 1篇 |
1986年 | 3篇 |
1985年 | 1篇 |
1984年 | 3篇 |
1983年 | 1篇 |
1981年 | 2篇 |
1979年 | 3篇 |
1978年 | 1篇 |
1977年 | 1篇 |
1975年 | 1篇 |
1974年 | 2篇 |
1972年 | 1篇 |
1971年 | 1篇 |
1970年 | 2篇 |
1969年 | 1篇 |
1968年 | 1篇 |
1965年 | 2篇 |
1964年 | 1篇 |
1963年 | 2篇 |
1962年 | 2篇 |
1960年 | 2篇 |
1932年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有233条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
61.
Mother's open-ended questions and elaborative statements during reminiscing were analysed for their content (child agency, co-agency, non-social, and social context) in three cultural contexts. Participants were 115 mothers and their 4-year-old children: 35 dyads from Berlin, Germany, 42 from Stockholm, Sweden, and 38 from Tallinn, Estonia. Across samples the most prominent content was talk about non-social context followed by co-agency and child agency. Tallinn mothers asked the children to talk about themselves, and Berlin mothers asked the children to talk about themselves together with other people, more frequently than they talked about these contents themselves. The content was related to the cultural orientations of mothers assessed through questionnaires: the Berlin mothers whose independence/ interdependence ratio was higher talked less about other people and asked the children fewer questions about other people; the Stockholm mothers with a higher independence/interdependence ratio talked more about child agency. In Tallinn both correlations existed on a trend level. The results are discussed in the light of common conversational practices and mothers' orientation to independence and interdependence in these cultural contexts. 相似文献
62.
Aline Vater Michela Schröder-Abé Kathrin Ritter Babette Renneberg Lars Schulze Jennifer K. Bosson 《Journal of personality assessment》2013,95(3):301-308
The Narcissistic Personality Inventory (NPI) has dominated research on narcissism in the field of social and personality psychology. Surprisingly, it is unclear whether the NPI is useful for identifying pathological narcissism in patients with Narcissistic Personality Disorder (NPD). The goal of this study was to close this research gap. We used an extreme-group approach by including NPD patients and healthy controls and comparing their narcissism scores. We further investigated whether explicit self-esteem (assessed with the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale) suppressed the relationship between group membership and NPI narcissism. According to our results, NPD patients do not score higher on the NPI in comparison to healthy controls. Analysis of indirect effects revealed that differences in NPI scores are suppressed by NPD patients’ low self-esteem. Our results indicate that the NPI is not a valid indicator of NPD, unless one controls for self-esteem. Implications for future research are discussed. 相似文献
63.
Lisa Schröder Heidi Keller Joscha Kärtner Astrid Kleis Monika Abels Relindis D. Yovsi 《Journal of cognition and development》2013,14(1):10-34
The present study examined conversations of 164 mothers from seven different cultural contexts when reminiscing with their 3-year-old children. We chose samples based on their sociodemographic profiles, which represented three different cultural models: (1) autonomy (urban middle-class families from Western societies), (2) relatedness (rural farming families from non-Western societies), and (3) autonomy-relatedness (urban middle-class families from non-Western societies). The results showed that mothers from the autonomous contexts predominantly adopted an elaborative-evaluative reminiscing style (variable-oriented approach) and pattern (person-oriented approach). Mothers from the relational contexts mainly adopted a repetitive reminiscing style and pattern. There was greater heterogeneity for style variables among mothers from autonomous-relational contexts; in addition, the person-oriented approach revealed that the majority of mothers from autonomous-relational contexts showed hybrid style patterns. Thus, the cultural models, and their respective orientations towards autonomy and relatedness, were reflected in the way mothers reminisced with their children. The children's provision of memory elaborations was high in the autonomous contexts, low in the relational contexts, and moderate in the autonomous-relational contexts. Across contexts, maternal evaluations prompted children to contribute memory elaborations. Maternal elaborations were an additional predictor for children's memory, but only for families from the relational cultural model. 相似文献
64.
Bahar Tunçgenç Annette Hohenberger Hannes Rakoczy 《Journal of cognition and development》2013,14(1):44-54
Two studies investigated young 2- and 3-year-old Turkish children's developing understanding of normativity and freedom to act in games. As expected, children, especially 3-year-olds, protested more when there was a norm violation than when there was none. Surprisingly, however, no decrease in normative protest was observed even when the actor violated the norms due to a physical constraint, and not due to unwillingness. The increase in helping responses in this case lends support to the idea that at these ages, children could not yet incorporate an actor's freedom to act in line with his will as they respond to norm transgressions. The results of the two studies are discussed in the light of two general research issues: a) the importance of cross-cultural research, and b) the interaction of the cognitive system with the emotional-empathic system in development. 相似文献
65.
Hannes Leitgeb 《Synthese》2011,180(2):265-299
This is a personal, incomplete, and very informal take on the role of logic in general philosophy of science, which is aimed
at a broader audience. We defend and advertise the application of logical methods in philosophy of science, starting with
the beginnings in the Vienna Circle and ending with some more recent logical developments. 相似文献
66.
Schröter H Fiedler A Miller J Ulrich R 《Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance》2011,37(5):1361-1368
In a simple reaction time (RT) experiment, visual stimuli were stereoscopically presented either to one eye (single stimulation) or to both eyes (redundant stimulation), with brightness matched for single and redundant stimulations. Redundant stimulation resulted in two separate percepts when noncorresponding retinal areas were stimulated, whereas it resulted in a single fused percept when corresponding areas were stimulated. With stimulation of noncorresponding areas, mean RT was shorter to redundant than to single stimulation, replicating the redundant signals effect (RSE) commonly found with visual stimuli. With stimulation of corresponding areas, however, no RSE was observed. This suggests that the RSE is driven by the number of percepts rather than by the number of stimulated receptors or sensory organs. These results are consistent with previous findings in the auditory modality and have implications for models of the RSE. 相似文献
67.
Famously, Frank P. Ramsey suggested a test for the acceptability of conditionals. Recently, David Chalmers and Alan Hájek
(2007) have criticized a qualitative variant of the Ramsey test for indicative conditionals. In this paper we argue for the
following three claims: (i) Chalmers and Hájek are right that the variant of the Ramsey test that they attack is not the correct
way of spelling out an acceptability test for indicative conditionals. But there is a suppositional variant of the Ramsey
test which is still stated in purely qualitative terms, which avoids the problems, and which looks correct. (ii) While the
variant of the Ramsey test that Chalmers and Hájek criticize is not correct, it is still a good approximation of a correct
formulation of the Ramsey test which may be usefully employed in various contexts. (iii) The variant of the Ramsey test that
Chalmers and Hájek suggest as a substitute for the deficient version of the Ramsey test is itself subject to worries similar
to those raised by Chalmers and Hájek, if it is given a non-suppositional interpretation. 相似文献
68.
In this study, the authors investigated leader generativity as a moderator of the relationships between leader age, leader-member exchange, and three criteria of leadership success (follower perceptions of leader effectiveness, follower satisfaction with leader, and follower extra effort). Data came from 128 university professors paired with one research assistant each. Results showed positive relationships between leader age and leader generativity, and negative relationships between leader age and follower perceptions of leader effectiveness and follower extra effort. Consistent with expectations based on leadership categorization theory, leader generativity moderated the relationships between leader age and all three criteria of leadership success, such that leaders high in generativity were better able to maintain high levels of leadership success at higher ages than leaders low in generativity. Finally, results of mediated moderation analyses showed that leader-member exchange quality mediated these moderating effects. The findings suggest that, in combination, leader age and the age-related construct of generativity importantly influence leadership processes and outcomes. 相似文献
69.
70.
Journal of Philosophical Logic - We investigate how to assign probabilities to sentences that contain a type-free truth predicate. These probability values track how often a sentence is satisfied... 相似文献