首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   516篇
  免费   44篇
  国内免费   78篇
  2024年   2篇
  2023年   8篇
  2022年   14篇
  2021年   13篇
  2020年   36篇
  2019年   21篇
  2018年   21篇
  2017年   30篇
  2016年   24篇
  2015年   16篇
  2014年   16篇
  2013年   51篇
  2012年   22篇
  2011年   32篇
  2010年   25篇
  2009年   28篇
  2008年   34篇
  2007年   41篇
  2006年   30篇
  2005年   28篇
  2004年   22篇
  2003年   18篇
  2002年   20篇
  2001年   11篇
  2000年   4篇
  1999年   16篇
  1998年   13篇
  1997年   5篇
  1996年   8篇
  1995年   9篇
  1994年   4篇
  1993年   3篇
  1992年   1篇
  1991年   2篇
  1986年   3篇
  1985年   2篇
  1984年   2篇
  1983年   1篇
  1979年   1篇
  1977年   1篇
排序方式: 共有638条查询结果,搜索用时 203 毫秒
291.
空间选择性注意在上下视野间的转移   总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2  
本实验研究空间选择性注意在上下视野间的转移及其是否受知觉空间的影响。实验采用Posner的位置线索化实验范式和两种深度线索背景,测量分辨任务的反应时(RTs)。实验发现:线索有效时的反应时比线索无效时的反应时短,而且线索对上视野刺激的反应时的影响较大。知觉空间中的深度线索对于注意在上下视野中转移没有显著影响。这些结果表明,空间选择性注意在上下视野转移存在不对称性,并且主要由物理空间决定。  相似文献   
292.
Past research on the theories of self‐construal and individualism‐collectivism in cross‐cultural contexts presents inconsistent and inconclusive results. Some researchers have seriously questioned the validity of major instruments measuring self‐construal across cultural groups. To address the validity issue, this study developed quantitative measures from ethnographic data. In five scenarios mapping self to close‐other boundaries, 171 Anglo‐Canadians and 224 Mainland Chinese were asked to make a decision and offer a reason for the decision. Two intriguing findings emerged from the data. (1) In comparison with Anglo‐Canadians, Mainland Chinese were more likely to share material belongings with close‐others and less likely to share their thoughts/opinions. The first part of this finding provides unequivocal support for the theories of self‐construal and individualism‐collectivism, whereas the latter part challenges an important assumption of these theories, which contends that collectivists should be more likely than individualists to share everything they own (including opinions) with close‐others. This unconventional finding proposes the division of material belongings and thoughts/opinions sharing of the self‐other boundary in future cross‐cultural self‐construal research. (2) There were significant differences in the reasons Canadians and Chinese offered for what they would or would not do in a specific situation. For example, the reasons for not telling the truth about a roommate's nonmatching outfit were “tastes differ from person to person” for a Canadian and “I don't tell others what I think of them” for a Chinese. The Canadians clearly show respect for the other's personal preference and the Chinese were thinking “what can I benefit from telling her the truth?” It was reasoned that underneath the giving and generous Chinese lies a shrewd mind, and underneath the frank Canadian lies a materialistic mind. In conclusion, this article contributes to the field in that it reports pioneering research, via both qualitative and quantitative means, on sharing material belongings and opinions/thoughts in samples from individualistic and collectivistic cultures. The findings of this study illustrate, specify, and challenge the universal utility of the theories of self‐construal and individualism‐collectivism.  相似文献   
293.
This study investigated the effects of programme context on memory for humorous television advertisements in South Korean participants. Humorous and nonhumorous Korean advertisements were embedded within two programme contexts: humorous and nonhumorous. When the programme ratings of humour, enjoyment and involvement were higher, unaided recall was poorer. In addition, unaided recall of the advertisements was better when they were embedded within a nonhumorous programme. However, there was no significant programme‐advertisement interaction effect. Overall, both free and cued recall were higher for humorous advertisements than for the nonhumorous advertisements. The findings are discussed in terms of cultural differences and changes in television programmes and advertising over time.Copyright © 2017 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.  相似文献   
294.
295.
With the frequent occurrence of campus violence, scholars have devoted increasing attention to college students’ aggression. This study aims to estimate the prevalence of aggression in Chinese university students and identify factors that could influence their aggression. We can thus find methods to reduce the incidence of college students’ aggression in the future. A multi-stage stratified sampling procedure was used to select university students (N = 4565) aged 16–25 years in Harbin. The Aggression Questionnaire, the Adolescent Self-Rating Life Events Checklist and the Social Support Revalued Scale were used to collect data. Females reported lower levels of aggression than males (p < .001). A multiple linear regression analysis was conducted to determine the influence of factors of aggression, and the model was highly significant (R2 = .233, Ad R2 = .230, p < .01). The results show that the aggression is affected by gender, family-level and school-level variables. Aggression scores are significantly correlated with not only family-level or school-level variables independently, but their combination as well. We find that the risk factors for aggression include a dissatisfying profession, higher levels of study pressure, poor parental relationships, poor interpersonal relationships, the presence of siblings, punishment, health maladjustment, less subjective support, and lower levels of utilization of social support.  相似文献   
296.
Müler-Lyer错觉作用机制的眼动研究   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
使用美国应用科学实验室生产的504型眼动仪,以错觉量和眼动特征为指标,考察了M櫣ller-Lyer(ML)错觉的产生及其作用过程。结果表明:(1)在ML错觉中,较长箭杆促进长度的准确判断,较长箭翼干扰长度的准确判断。减弱ML图形的总长度线索有助于降低错觉量;(3)箭杆与箭翼的交互作用对注视时间产生显著影响;(3)被试通过整合两条矛盾的线索进行长度判断,实验结果支持矛盾线索理论。  相似文献   
297.
以124名本科生为被试,通过划分利手程度(实验一)与添加眼动刺激(实验二)的方法,系统考察了大脑半球互动对创造性科学问题提出能力的影响作用。结果发现:(1)与强右利手被试相比,混合利手被试在创造性科学问题提出能力测验上的得分更高;(2)与非眼动组被试相比,眼动组被试在创造性科学问题提出能力测验上的表现更好;(3)大脑半球互动与问题情境在灵活性品质上存在交互作用,大脑半球互动对开放性问题灵活性品质的影响大于封闭性问题。  相似文献   
298.
The present study examined the behavioral effects of the first-person possessive pronoun. In each trial, a noun (e.g. cup or bread) was presented to participants after visual presentation of a possessive pronoun “wo de” (Chinese for “my”) or “ta de” (Chinese for “his”), which formed ownership. Half participants were assigned to contextual encoding (CE) condition in which they were required to judge whether they liked the item expressed by a noun from the first or third-person perspective. The rest were assigned to perceptual encoding (PE) condition in which they were asked to judge what color the noun was. A subsequent recall test was performed. The results showed that there were significant memory and response advantages for nouns in “my” ownership under both conditions. The results were discussed with reference to self-specificity and other effects in the current study.  相似文献   
299.
The 2008 recession reportedly led to the demise of conspicuous consumption with wealthy consumers abandoning luxury goods that prominently display their brands for more subdued designs. Utilizing data collected before and in the midst of the recession from designer handbag manufacturers, Louis Vuitton and Gucci, we find products introduced during the recession actually display the brand far more prominently than those products withdrawn. Data from Hermès and luxury ads in Vogue magazine also indicate manufacturers did not tone things down. Our results suggest conspicuous consumption endures in recessions; consumers who do not exit the luxury goods market are still interested in logo‐laden products.  相似文献   
300.
本文认为,在柏拉图<普罗泰哥拉篇>中潜藏的叙事动力是勇敢美德的殊异性.而勇敢美德的殊异性与美德整体性学说是柏拉图对话尤其是<普罗泰哥拉篇>和<拉克斯篇>中的一对相关性理论.二者分别从哲学和政治的角度表明,普罗泰哥拉式的美德仅仅是一种可以传授的技艺,它既不是无法技术化的勇敢,也不是理念化的知识.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号