全文获取类型
收费全文 | 444篇 |
免费 | 18篇 |
专业分类
462篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 5篇 |
2022年 | 3篇 |
2020年 | 6篇 |
2019年 | 12篇 |
2018年 | 12篇 |
2017年 | 23篇 |
2016年 | 17篇 |
2015年 | 13篇 |
2014年 | 6篇 |
2013年 | 51篇 |
2012年 | 19篇 |
2011年 | 22篇 |
2010年 | 11篇 |
2009年 | 13篇 |
2008年 | 23篇 |
2007年 | 27篇 |
2006年 | 22篇 |
2005年 | 6篇 |
2004年 | 4篇 |
2003年 | 9篇 |
2002年 | 9篇 |
2001年 | 3篇 |
2000年 | 8篇 |
1999年 | 6篇 |
1995年 | 4篇 |
1994年 | 3篇 |
1992年 | 6篇 |
1991年 | 4篇 |
1990年 | 5篇 |
1989年 | 6篇 |
1988年 | 4篇 |
1987年 | 4篇 |
1986年 | 4篇 |
1985年 | 3篇 |
1984年 | 7篇 |
1982年 | 2篇 |
1980年 | 5篇 |
1978年 | 5篇 |
1977年 | 4篇 |
1976年 | 5篇 |
1975年 | 5篇 |
1974年 | 7篇 |
1973年 | 4篇 |
1972年 | 3篇 |
1971年 | 3篇 |
1970年 | 7篇 |
1969年 | 5篇 |
1968年 | 5篇 |
1967年 | 5篇 |
1966年 | 2篇 |
排序方式: 共有462条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
221.
222.
Christopher Hamilton 《Ethical Theory and Moral Practice》2008,11(2):181-195
Raimond Gaita’s work in moral philosophy is unusual and important in focusing on the concept of sainthood. Drawing partly
on the work of George Orwell, and partly on the life and work of Simone Weil, as well as on further material, I argue that
Gaita’s use of this notion to help make sense of the concept of human preciousness is unconvincing, not least because he does
not properly explore the figure and psychology of the saint in any detail. I relatedly argue that the notion of human preciousness
in question is implausible and, in some ways, sentimental. I also explore Gaita’s concept of “speaking personally” in moral
philosophy, and suggest that matters here are a great deal more complicated than he supposes.
相似文献
Christopher HamiltonEmail: |
223.
Abstract. Engaging students in a course in the Sociology of Religion can be a challenge, particularly when working with student populations in a homogeneous region of the country who have limited experience with religious diversity. We approached the course from a sociological/anthropological perspective, requiring each student to complete an in‐depth participation/observation research experience and write an ethnographic account of a religion or belief system different from his or her own. While other instructors have used a similar pedagogy, using ethnography with our student population was generally successful as a learning and writing tool. 相似文献
224.
225.
Turner TH Goldstein J Hamilton JM Jacobson M Pirogovsky E Peavy G Corey-Bloom J 《Journal of motor behavior》2011,43(4):295-302
Initiation and inhibition of saccadic eye movements has been shown to be impaired in patients with Huntington's disease (HD) and premanifest gene carriers (PMGC), and may provide biomarkers useful in tracking phenotypic change. Computerized behavioral tests of prosaccade latency and disinhibition presented to 31 non-gene carriers (NGC), 25 PMGC, and 12 HD patients. These tests provided quantitative performance measures without use of eye-tracking equipment. Significant differences on saccade tests were found, with PMGC intermediate between NGC and HD patients. Saccade latency discriminated PMGC from NGC, whereas saccade disinhibition discriminated PMGC from HD patients. Results suggest utility of behavioral saccade measures as premanifest indicators of phenoconversion in HD. 相似文献
226.
Gender stereotyping and under-representation of girls and women have been documented in children’s picture books in the past, in the hope that improvements would follow. Most researchers have analyzed award-winning books. We explored sexism in top selling books from 2001 and a 7-year sample of Caldecott award-winning books, for a total of 200 books. There were nearly twice as many male as female title and main characters. Male characters appeared 53% more times in illustrations. Female main characters nurtured more than male main characters did, and they were seen in more indoor than outdoor scenes. Occupations were gender stereotyped, and more women than men appeared to have no paid occupation. Few differences were found between Caldecott award books and other books. A comparison of our book sample to 1980s and 1990s books did not reveal reduced sexism. The persistence of sexism in picture books and implications for children and parents are discussed. 相似文献
227.
228.
The parents of a 6-year-old disruptive boy were observed interacting with their son during 20 assessment-oriented, 10- minute experimental sessions. An experimental design consisting of alternately arranged baseline and self-monitoring conditions was utilized. Following the reversal- to- baseline phase, parents were given specialized training in self-recording positive parental praise. Results indicated that (a) self-monitoring of positive verbalizations was an effective means of producing reactive changes in parental behavior, and (b) training in self-monitoring increased parent-observer accuracy scores to more than acceptable levels of agreement. These findings were discussed with regard to the simplicity of a parental self-observational intervention and recommendations were made for continuing investigations in the area.This research was supported by the University of Montana Research Grant and Fellowship Program. 相似文献
229.
Mark Salo Holly Forester-Miller W. Mark Hamilton 《Journal of counseling and development : JCD》1996,75(2):174-176
This report contains data about ethical complaints and inquiries processed by the American Counseling Association (ACA) Ethics Committee from 1995 to 1996. This report also includes a discussion of the findings of the committee, information about the approval of the new ACA Code of Ethics and Standards of Practice, and educational efforts undertaken by the committee. 相似文献
230.
Maternal sensitivity during infancy and subsequent life events relate to attachment representation at early adulthood. 总被引:4,自引:0,他引:4
A prospective longitudinal research study of 86 prematurely born children from birth to age 18 years provided empirical evidence for continuity from infancy experience to representations of attachment at age 18 years. Young adults whose representation of attachment was dismissing had been objectively observed during infancy, 16-17 years earlier, to receive less sensitive maternal care than those infants who were later judged at early adulthood to have secure or preoccupied representations. Infancy experience alone did not differentiate young adults with secure representations from those with preoccupied representations. Rather, adverse life events through age 12, particularly parental divorce, reduced the likelihood of secure representations and increased the likelihood of preoccupied representations. The absence of adverse life events did not increase the likelihood of security for those who had not experienced early sensitive caregiving. 相似文献