全文获取类型
收费全文 | 49648篇 |
免费 | 899篇 |
国内免费 | 5篇 |
专业分类
50552篇 |
出版年
2019年 | 483篇 |
2018年 | 633篇 |
2017年 | 679篇 |
2016年 | 757篇 |
2015年 | 558篇 |
2014年 | 677篇 |
2013年 | 3524篇 |
2012年 | 1203篇 |
2011年 | 1262篇 |
2010年 | 820篇 |
2009年 | 804篇 |
2008年 | 1195篇 |
2007年 | 1074篇 |
2006年 | 990篇 |
2005年 | 924篇 |
2004年 | 871篇 |
2003年 | 890篇 |
2002年 | 926篇 |
2001年 | 1277篇 |
2000年 | 1251篇 |
1999年 | 930篇 |
1998年 | 484篇 |
1997年 | 492篇 |
1996年 | 471篇 |
1992年 | 908篇 |
1991年 | 863篇 |
1990年 | 868篇 |
1989年 | 842篇 |
1988年 | 803篇 |
1987年 | 784篇 |
1986年 | 843篇 |
1985年 | 867篇 |
1984年 | 744篇 |
1983年 | 685篇 |
1982年 | 504篇 |
1981年 | 573篇 |
1979年 | 806篇 |
1978年 | 623篇 |
1977年 | 548篇 |
1976年 | 546篇 |
1975年 | 710篇 |
1974年 | 843篇 |
1973年 | 890篇 |
1972年 | 708篇 |
1971年 | 723篇 |
1970年 | 676篇 |
1969年 | 711篇 |
1968年 | 885篇 |
1967年 | 816篇 |
1966年 | 818篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
861.
JOHN W. MYERS JETTIE McWILLIAMS JOHN FLANDERS JERALDINE KOHUT 《Journal of Employment Counseling》1983,20(2):51-60
This project's goals were to sensitize job service counselors to the needs and employment problems of older rural job-seekers and to develop a model in-service program. 相似文献
862.
863.
Peter H Schönemann 《Journal of mathematical psychology》1983,27(3):311-324
In an earlier note, a new metric for bounded response scales (MBR) was introduced which resembles the city-block metric but is bounded above. It was suggested the MBR may be more appropriate than minkowski metrics for data obtained with bounded response scales. In this article, some formal properties of the MBR are investigated and it is shown that it is indeed a metric. Empirical predictions are then derived from the MBR and contrasted with those of a “monotonicity hypothesis,” which holds that dissimilarity judgements tend to be biased towards overestimation of larger distances, and with the predictions of the minkowski metrics, which imply additivity of collinear segments. Some empirical results are presented which contradict the monotonicity hypothesis and the minkowski metrics, and favor the MBR. Finally, the logic used to motivate the MBR is invoked to define a subadditive concatenation for bounded norms in the one-dimensional case, which may be useful in psychophysical work where the upper bounds are often real, rather than due to the response scale. This concatenation predicts understimation for doubling and overestimation for halving and middling tasks. 相似文献
864.
It is shown that deterministic models can compete effectively with stochastic models in summarizing concept identification behavior. Three groups of deterministic models are examined. Examination of individual learners' trial by trial behavior in a concept experiment shows: (1) One person exhibited behavior consistent with a Hypothesis Permutation (HP) model despite being a nonlearner who showed no evidence of improvement over a period of 24 trials. However, when all 50 persons studied in each of two treatment groups were examined, only 22 members of one group and 10 of the other showed no inconsistencies with deterministic local consistency assumptions. (2) Certain deterministic computer programs could find at least one satisfactory order for predicting all responses by 18 of the 22 consistent solvers and 6 of the 10 consistent solvers, respectively, in the two groups just mentioned. For these 24 persons, then, a less restrictive deterministic model is adequate than for the others. (3) Those 38 original members of the first treatment group who met a stringent learning criterion were compared with respect to predictions generated by stochastic and mathematized deterministic models. One deterministic model (RSS-U 9-state) is in some respects the best of the models examined, but this success is a partial reflection of estimating eight parameters from the data. 相似文献
865.
Mary K. Enright Carolyn K. Rovee-Collier Jeffrey W. Fagen Karen Caniglia 《Journal of experimental child psychology》1983,36(2):209-225
Long-term retention of operant footkicking acquired in the mobile conjugate reinforcement paradigm was assessed as a function of the distribution of training time. In the first study, 3-month-old infants were trained for either one 18-min session or for two 9-min or three 6-min sessions separated by 24-hr intervals. All infants exhibited retention during a test administered immediately after training, but only those trained in a single session continued to perform the conditioned response during cued-recall tests 7 or 14 days later. Infants trained in three sessions showed no evidence of remembering the contingency even after a week. A warm-up decrement, seen in the day-to-day performance of infants in the distributed conditions, was eliminated in the second study by the interpolation of a nonreinforcement period at the outset of daily sessions. This procedure also enhanced long-term retention such that infants trained in three 6-min session now remembered the contingency for 14 days and did not differ from infants who had received a single 18-min session. Whether distributed training facilitates or impairs long-term retention appears to depend on the opportunity for infants to acquire a sufficient number (or kind) of effective retrieval cues during original learning. 相似文献
866.
HARVEY W. TRIMMER 《Journal of Employment Counseling》1983,20(3):128-138
This article uses Jones' comparative table for the typology of Holland and the Guide for Occupational Exploration to extend the relationships to include the occupational themes and work roles of Jackson and Kuder. 相似文献
867.
Chusmir LH 《Journal of personality assessment》1983,47(1):29-35
Measures of need Achievement, need Affiliation, and need Power were obtained for 124 working managers (62 female, 62 male) using both McClelland's traditional male-oriented thematic apperception test (TAT) and a set of picture cues balanced-as-to-sex. Two-way analysis of variance was performed in a 2 x 2 factorial design (sex by version of TAT). F tests were conducted to test significance at the .05 level. Results indicate both sets are interchangeable in determining need Achievement and need Power distinctions between managerial women and men, Absolute scores are also comparable. For need Affiliation, however, motivation scores are not comparable and interchangeability is clouded, suggesting cue modification may be needed. 相似文献
868.
Although the Bender-Gestalt Visual Motor Test has been a popular and influential clinical assessment instrument for the last half century, authoritative opinion and a substantial research literature are often inconsistent or contradictory. This paper documents variations in designs, administration procedures, forms, and applications to particular populations. A careful review of scoring systems indicates the current status of objective approaches as contrasted with interpretation that is global, intuitive, and subjective. Some implications of these variations and interpretive procedures for training and assessment practice are discussed. The major conclusions of the review are summarized in seven points. 相似文献
869.
The following measures were obtained from 42 student volunteers: the General and the Disinhibition subscales of the Sensation Seeking Scale (Form IV), the Reducer-Augmenter Scale, and the Absolute Auditory Threshold. General sensation seeking correlated significantly with the Reducer-Augmenter Scale, r(40) = .59, p less than .001, and the Absolute Auditory Threshold, r(40) = .45, p less than .005. Both results proved general across sex. These findings, that high-sensation seekers tend to be reducers and to lack sensitivity to weak stimulation, were interpreted as supporting strength-of-the-nervous-system theory more than the formulation of Zuckerman and his associates. 相似文献
870.
J Brockner M Gardner J Bierman T Mahan B Thomas W Weiss L Winters A Mitchell 《Journal of personality and social psychology》1983,45(1):199-209
The present experiments were designed to study the conditions under which failure would enhance or inhibit subsequent task performance. Based on the theory of Wortman and Brehm (1975), it was expected that small amounts of failure would produce reactance (manifested by improved performance at a subsequent task), whereas large amounts would lead to learned helplessness (i.e., impaired later performance). It was further expected that individual differences in self-esteem and private self-consciousness would serve as moderator variables for the above effects. In Experiment 1, subjects were exposed to either a small amount of failure or no failure before working on an anagrams task. As predicted, subjects high in self-consciousness, who have shown greater reactance arousal in attitude change studies, performed better on the anagrams task than subjects low in self-consciousness in the small-failure condition, but not in the no-failure condition. Further analyses revealed that this Self-Consciousness X Small Failure interaction was attributable to the performance data of the low, but not the high self-esteem subjects. Experiment 2 was designed to replicate and extend these results. Subjects were pretreated with either a small amount of failure, an extended amount of failure, or no failure before working on the anagrams task. A significant Self-Esteem X Helplessness Training interaction emerged. Relative to the no-failure condition, in which the two self-esteem groups did not differ, low self-esteem participants (low SEs) performed marginally better than did high self-esteem individuals (high SEs) in the small-failure condition but significantly worse than high SEs in the extended-failure condition. The effect of private self-consciousness was considerably weaker in this study, possibly because the sample included few low SEs (who are especially influenced by self-focused attention) who were also relatively low in self-consciousness. Questionnaire data from Experiment 2 were consistent with the notion that enhanced performance reflected reactance, whereas impaired performance signified helplessness. 相似文献