全文获取类型
收费全文 | 20764篇 |
免费 | 14篇 |
专业分类
20778篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 3篇 |
2022年 | 5篇 |
2021年 | 7篇 |
2020年 | 20篇 |
2019年 | 12篇 |
2018年 | 3486篇 |
2017年 | 2827篇 |
2016年 | 2260篇 |
2015年 | 204篇 |
2014年 | 84篇 |
2013年 | 120篇 |
2012年 | 573篇 |
2011年 | 2402篇 |
2010年 | 2524篇 |
2009年 | 1484篇 |
2008年 | 1709篇 |
2007年 | 2175篇 |
2006年 | 40篇 |
2005年 | 221篇 |
2004年 | 178篇 |
2003年 | 117篇 |
2002年 | 64篇 |
2001年 | 35篇 |
2000年 | 55篇 |
1999年 | 18篇 |
1998年 | 25篇 |
1997年 | 20篇 |
1996年 | 12篇 |
1995年 | 5篇 |
1994年 | 4篇 |
1993年 | 3篇 |
1992年 | 3篇 |
1991年 | 4篇 |
1990年 | 11篇 |
1988年 | 4篇 |
1986年 | 10篇 |
1985年 | 7篇 |
1984年 | 4篇 |
1983年 | 2篇 |
1982年 | 4篇 |
1981年 | 3篇 |
1979年 | 2篇 |
1976年 | 3篇 |
1975年 | 3篇 |
1974年 | 2篇 |
1973年 | 5篇 |
1971年 | 2篇 |
1969年 | 2篇 |
1968年 | 3篇 |
1967年 | 2篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
831.
Graduate employment status and health: a longitudinal analysis of the transition from student 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
The prevalence of unemployment and underemployment was explored longitudinally in 248 recent graduates (73 males and 175 females)
over an 18–24 month transition from final year student to 9–12 months post-graduation. Over this period changes in levels
of psychological distress, health behaviours, social support, optimism and achievement motivation were measured. Results show
that both unemployment and underemployment have deleterious effects on psychological and physical health, social support,
optimism and achievement motivation.
相似文献
Tony CassidyEmail: |
832.
This paper reports a study of a multi-agent model of working memory (WM) in the context of Boolean concept learning. The model aims to assess the compressibility of information processed in WM. Concept complexity is described as a function of communication resources (i.e., the number of agents and the structure of communication between agents) required in WM to learn a target concept. This model has been successfully applied in measuring learning times for three-dimensional (3D) concepts (Mathy and Bradmetz in Curr Psychol Cognit 22(1):41-82, 2004). In this previous study, learning time was found to be a function of compression time. To assess the effect of decompression time, this paper presents an extended intra-conceptual study of response times for two- and 3D concepts. Response times are measured in recognition phases. The model explains why the time required to compress a sample of examples into a rule is directly linked to the time to decompress this rule when categorizing examples. Three experiments were conducted with 65, 49, and 84 undergraduate students who were given Boolean concept learning tasks in two and three dimensions (also called rule-based classification tasks). The results corroborate the metric of decompression given by the multi-agent model, especially when the model is parameterized following static serial processing of information. Also, this static serial model better fits the patterns of response times than an exemplar-based model. 相似文献
833.
The present study aimed at investigating whether different aspects of performance on a prospective reproduction task with a standard duration of 2 s had different antecedents in the event-related potentials (ERPs) accompanying standard presentations and reproduced intervals. On each trial, first the standard duration was presented as an empty auditory interval. After a short delay, participants reproduced this interval by means of two button presses defining onset and offset of the reproduced interval. About 25 participants were divided into groups of poor and good time estimators, once based on the coefficient of variation of their reproduced durations, and once based on the absolute error score. Interestingly, for both performance measures differences between participants were accompanied by ERP differences during standard presentations rather than reproduction itself. While larger P300 amplitudes evoked by standard interval offset predicted good reproduction performance in terms of a small coefficient of variation, small absolute error scores were accompanied by larger fronto-central negative slow wave during standard presentations. The results suggest an important role of attention to both the critical events that delimit the to-be-estimated temporal intervals, and to the passing of time between these events. 相似文献
834.
Bouville M 《Science and engineering ethics》2008,14(1):111-120
Many engineering ethics classes and textbooks introduce theories such as utilitarianism and Kantianism (and most others draw from these theories without mentioning them explicitly). Yet using ethical theories to teach engineering ethics is not devoid of difficulty. First, their status is unclear (should one pick a single theory or use them all? does it make a difference?) Also, textbooks generally assume or fallaciously ‘prove’ that egoism (or even simply accounting for one’s interests) is wrong. Further, the drawbacks of ethical theories are underestimated and the theories are also otherwise misrepresented to make them more suitable for engineering ethics as the authors construe it, viz. the ‘moral reasoning’ process. Stating in what various theories disagree would allow the students to frame the problem more productively in terms of motive–consequence or society–individual dichotomies rather than in terms of Kant–utilitarian. 相似文献
835.
Varelius J 《Science and engineering ethics》2008,14(1):65-76
Services of ethics consultants are nowadays commonly used in such various spheres of life as engineering, public administration, business, law, health care, journalism, and scientific research. It has however been maintained that use of ethics consultants is incompatible with personal autonomy; in moral matters individuals should be allowed to make their own decisions. The problem this criticism refers to can be conceived of as a conflict between the professional autonomy of ethics experts and the autonomy of the persons they serve. This paper addresses this conflict and maintains that when the nature of both ethics consultation and individual autonomy is properly understood, the professional autonomy of ethics experts is compatible with the autonomy of the persons they assist. 相似文献
836.
Krishnasamy ST 《Science and engineering ethics》2008,14(2):291-293
The object of this communication is to reflect on the recent article by James Stieb in relation to an earlier article by Selvan. 相似文献
837.
838.
839.
Simone Gozzano 《Axiomathes》2008,18(1):117-126
In recent times, Evans’ idea that mental states could have non-conceptual contents has been attacked. McDowell (Mind and World,
1994) and Brewer (Perception and reason, 1999) have both argued that that notion does not have any epistemological role because notions such as justification or evidential support, that might relate mental contents to each other, must be framed in conceptual terms. On his side, Brewer has argued that
instead of non-conceptual content we should consider demonstrative concepts that have the same fine grainess of non-conceptual contents while having conceptual structure. In what follows I will argue
that, first, that the notion of demonstrative concept is not viable and, second, that there is an epistemological role for non-conceptual content.
相似文献
Simone GozzanoEmail: |
840.
Dima Mohammed 《Argumentation》2008,22(3):377-393
This paper aims at creating an adequate theoretical basis for a systematic integration of institutional insights into the
pragma-dialectical analysis of argumentative exchanges that occur in institutionalised contexts. The argumentative practice
of Prime Minister’s Question Time in the British House of Commons is examined, as a case in point, in order to illustrate
how the knowledge of the characteristics of an institution, its rules and conventions can be integrated into the pragma-dialectical
analysis. The paper highlights the role that theoretical concepts and tools such as strategic manoeuvring, argumentative activity
types and dialectical profiles play in this integration.
相似文献
Dima MohammedEmail: |