首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   6791篇
  免费   348篇
  国内免费   4篇
  2023年   72篇
  2022年   117篇
  2021年   135篇
  2020年   187篇
  2019年   194篇
  2018年   324篇
  2017年   325篇
  2016年   311篇
  2015年   210篇
  2014年   232篇
  2013年   889篇
  2012年   507篇
  2011年   500篇
  2010年   320篇
  2009年   230篇
  2008年   397篇
  2007年   328篇
  2006年   291篇
  2005年   263篇
  2004年   168篇
  2003年   169篇
  2002年   167篇
  2001年   69篇
  2000年   87篇
  1999年   53篇
  1998年   34篇
  1997年   27篇
  1996年   28篇
  1995年   23篇
  1994年   18篇
  1993年   22篇
  1992年   32篇
  1991年   23篇
  1990年   29篇
  1989年   28篇
  1988年   30篇
  1987年   19篇
  1986年   19篇
  1985年   14篇
  1984年   19篇
  1983年   18篇
  1982年   14篇
  1979年   17篇
  1978年   13篇
  1976年   15篇
  1973年   10篇
  1971年   10篇
  1970年   10篇
  1968年   11篇
  1965年   10篇
排序方式: 共有7143条查询结果,搜索用时 31 毫秒
41.
Pain experienced by Black individuals is systematically underestimated, and recent studies have shown that part of this bias is rooted in perceptual factors. We used Reverse Correlation to estimate visual representations of the pain expression in Black and White faces, in participants originating from both Western and African countries. Groups of raters were then asked to evaluate the presence of pain and other emotions in these representations. A second group of White raters then evaluated those same representations placed over a neutral background face (50% White; 50% Black). Image-based analyses show significant effects of culture and face ethnicity, but no interaction between the two factors. Western representations were more likely to be judged as expressing pain than African representations. For both cultural groups, raters also perceived more pain in White face representations than in Black face representations. However, when changing the background stimulus to the neutral background face, this effect of face ethnic profile disappeared. Overall, these results suggest that individuals have different expectations of how pain is expressed by Black and White individuals, and that cultural factors may explain a part of this phenomenon  相似文献   
42.
43.
44.
45.
46.
47.
Stig Fhanér 《Psychometrika》1966,31(2):267-269
It is shown that certain correlation measures suggested by information theory are simple functions of the determinants of the correlation matrices involved, if multivariate normality is assumed. This illuminates and in a sense strengthens Rozeboom's point in [3], that information theoretical statistics have classical correlational counterparts.  相似文献   
48.
49.
In the Critique of Judgement, Kant, despite his strong condemnation of rhetoric, introduces the figure of hypotyposis at the very moment he sets out to tackle the philosophical problem of presentation as such. This study holds that this choice of the rhetorical term is not fortuitous. Its connotations of vivid illustration, synopsis, and moral grandeur serve Kant in arguing that, on a transcendental level, presentation secures the mind's life, unity, and self-affection. Although of rhetorical origin, hypotyposis is thus shown to link up with a specifically philosophical meaning of the term in the writings of Aristotle.  相似文献   
50.
When an observer views a moving scene binocularly, both motion parallax and binocular disparity provide depth information. In Experiments lA-1C, we measured sensitivity to surface curvature when these depth cues were available either individually or simultaneously. When the depth cues yielded comparable sensitivity to surface curvature, we found that curvature detection was easier with the cues present simultaneously, rather than individually. For 2 of the 6 subjects, this effect was stronger when the component of frontal translation of the surface was vertical, rather than horizontal. No such anisotropy was found for the 4 other subjects. If a moving object is observed binocularly, the patterns of optic flow are different on the left and right retinae. We have suggested elsewhere (Cornilleau-Pérès & Droulez, in press) that this motion disparity might be used as avisual cue for the perception of a 3-D structure. Our model consisted in deriving binocular disparity from the left and right distributions of vertical velocities, rather than from luminous intensities, as has been done in classical studies on stereoscopic vision. The model led to some predictions concerning the detection of surface curvature from motion disparity in the presence or absence of intensity-based disparity (classically termedbinocular disparity). In a second set of experiments, we attempted to test these predictions, and we failed to validate our theoretical scheme from a physiological point of view.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号