首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   1833篇
  免费   71篇
  2023年   9篇
  2021年   12篇
  2020年   34篇
  2019年   41篇
  2018年   49篇
  2017年   43篇
  2016年   44篇
  2015年   22篇
  2014年   45篇
  2013年   207篇
  2012年   90篇
  2011年   76篇
  2010年   52篇
  2009年   55篇
  2008年   93篇
  2007年   100篇
  2006年   87篇
  2005年   76篇
  2004年   69篇
  2003年   67篇
  2002年   53篇
  2001年   31篇
  2000年   30篇
  1999年   37篇
  1998年   29篇
  1997年   29篇
  1996年   23篇
  1995年   20篇
  1994年   17篇
  1993年   21篇
  1992年   15篇
  1991年   18篇
  1990年   30篇
  1989年   19篇
  1988年   20篇
  1987年   18篇
  1986年   20篇
  1985年   16篇
  1984年   15篇
  1983年   12篇
  1982年   17篇
  1981年   18篇
  1980年   15篇
  1979年   19篇
  1978年   11篇
  1977年   10篇
  1976年   5篇
  1975年   8篇
  1974年   11篇
  1973年   7篇
排序方式: 共有1904条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
111.
112.
Gregory R 《Perception》2003,32(10):1155-1157
  相似文献   
113.
114.
A behavioral and computational treatment of change detection is reported. The behavioral task was to judge whether a single object substitution change occurred between two “flickering” 9-object scenes. Detection performance was found to vary with the similarity of the changing objects; object changes violating orientation and category yielded the fastest and most accurate detection responses. To account for these data, theBOLAR model was developed, which uses color, orientation, and scale selective filters to compute the visual dissimilarity between the pre- and postchange objects from the behavioral study. Relating the magnitude of the BOLAR difference signals to change detection performance revealed that object pairs estimated as visually least similar were the same object pairs most easily detected by observers. The BOLAR model advances change detection theory by (1) demonstrating that the visual similarity between the change patterns can account for much of the variability in change detection behavior, and (2) providing a computational technique for quantifying these visual similarity relationships for real-world objects.  相似文献   
115.
116.
117.
Gregory R. Peterson 《Zygon》1999,34(1):139-149
Beginning with the End represents an excellent collection of articles devoted to the thought of Wolfhart Pannenberg. This volume includes many of the most important thinkers in the science-religion dialogue and shows as well the importance and impact of Pannenberg's theology. This response addresses themes that surface in several of the articles: What is religion? What is science? What is theology? What is God? On some of these themes there is agreement, on others sharp disagreement. The conclusion also considers what this volume suggests about the future of Pannenberg's theology.  相似文献   
118.
119.
Many researchers have proposed that, for the purpose of recognition, human vision parses shapes into component parts. Precisely how is not yet known. The minima rule for silhouettes (Hoffman & Richards, 1984) defines boundary points at which to parse but does not tell how to use these points to cut silhouettes and, therefore, does not tell what the parts are. In this paper, we propose the short-cut rule, which states that, other things being equal, human vision prefers to use the shortest possible cuts to parse silhouettes. We motivate this rule, and the well-known Petter’s rule for modal completion, by the principle of transversality. We present five psychophysical experiments that test the short-cut rule, show that it successfully predicts part cuts that connect boundary points given by the minima rule, and show that it can also create new boundary points.  相似文献   
120.
In two experiments, we examined whether voluntary and reflexive saccades shared a common fixation disengagement mechanism, Participants were required to perform a variety of tasks, each requiring a different level of information processing of the display prior to execution of the saccade. In Experiment 1, participants executed either a prosaccade or an antisaccade upon detecting a stimulus array. In Experiment 2, participants executed a prosaccade to a stimulus array only if the array contained a target item. The target could be a line (easy search) or a digit (difficult search). The critical manipulation in both experiments was the relative timing between the removal of the fixation stimulus and the onset of the stimulus array. In both experiments, it was found that saccadic latencies were shortest when the fixation stimulus was removed before the onset of the stimulus array—a gap effect. It was concluded that reflexive and voluntary saccades share a common fixation disengagement mechanism that is largely independent of higher level cognitive processes.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号