首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   2510篇
  免费   107篇
  2023年   17篇
  2022年   9篇
  2021年   18篇
  2020年   39篇
  2019年   55篇
  2018年   66篇
  2017年   58篇
  2016年   62篇
  2015年   37篇
  2014年   57篇
  2013年   284篇
  2012年   103篇
  2011年   110篇
  2010年   67篇
  2009年   78篇
  2008年   112篇
  2007年   132篇
  2006年   97篇
  2005年   120篇
  2004年   99篇
  2003年   90篇
  2002年   73篇
  2001年   50篇
  2000年   45篇
  1999年   59篇
  1998年   42篇
  1997年   35篇
  1996年   27篇
  1995年   22篇
  1994年   22篇
  1993年   28篇
  1992年   24篇
  1991年   31篇
  1990年   39篇
  1989年   39篇
  1988年   25篇
  1987年   26篇
  1986年   30篇
  1985年   27篇
  1984年   21篇
  1983年   19篇
  1982年   18篇
  1981年   24篇
  1980年   21篇
  1979年   20篇
  1978年   12篇
  1977年   15篇
  1975年   11篇
  1974年   14篇
  1973年   12篇
排序方式: 共有2617条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
991.
This articleresponds to the comments ofTversky and Hemenway (1991), who criticized the logic, stimuli, and data analysis of Murphy (1991). It is argued here that their objections do not mitigate the conclusions drawn by Murphy. In particular, the objection that the stimuli were not natural enough to reveal differences between category levels seems to presuppose an answer to the question under investigation. However, further experimentation with other stimuli might resolve this issue empirically.  相似文献   
992.
Parts in object concepts: Experiments with artificial categories   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
Previous research has demonstrated that basic-level categories are associated with the parts of objects. Five experiments were conducted to investigate whether this correlation is caused by a psychological principle that requires basic concepts to have common parts. This hypothesis was investigated in the experiments by separating part and nonpart information in artificial categories. Basic-level structure was measured in two ways: as the level with the highest ratio of within-category similarity to between-category similarity, and as the level with the fastest categorization time in an object-identification task. The results revealed that basic-level structure could be found in categories that did not have parts in common. Furthermore, nonpart information, such as size, color, and texture, could both enhance the basic level and eliminate the basic-level advantage, depending on whether the nonpart information was consistent with or contradicted the part information. These results suggest that, psychologically, parts are neither necessary nor sufficient to form a basic level.  相似文献   
993.
Four interaction-perception perspectives of the fictitious target of a double-bind conflict were provided by four groups of subjects. The perspectives included perceptions of a daughter about herself and her father (the double-bind source), and her estimates of his perceptions of both himself and her. As predicted, the target saw herself as weak but “good”, and as cooperative and highly frustrated, while attributing all the opposite characteristics to the source, who was seen as strong but “bad”, and uncooperative and not frustrated. Further, it was found that she believed he saw himself as strong and very good, but frustrated and moderately cooperative. These observations are consistent with a hypothesis that double-bind experiences result in frustration and mixed feelings toward one's self and the source of the dilemma. It was also concluded that the target's belief that her father would not recognize her weakness, nor her cooperativeness, nor her frustration, and would disinterestedly evaluate her as neither good nor bad resulted from the inconsistent and contradictory communications which had contributed to the double bind.  相似文献   
994.
995.
Jones (1976) found that the accuracy of recall of the order in which a series of pictures was seen was dependent upon the way in which recall was cued. For the latter part of a series, sequential position was a more effective cue for the recall of the corresponding picture than the picture was for sequential position. Two experiments reported here (testing altogether 100 subjects) investigated whether this terminal asymmetry effect (TAE) arises at encoding or only at retrieval. It was found that the TAE was affected by the conditions of presentation of the series but not by the post-presentation factors studied.  相似文献   
996.
997.
998.
Systems for creating and running psychology experiments on an Apple II or IBM PC are described. Without programming, psychologists using the APT systems can construct and conduct an experiment, summarize the data, and revise the experiment. The systems are easily used by students but powerful enough to be used by psychologists in their research.  相似文献   
999.
1000.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号