首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   2337篇
  免费   93篇
  2430篇
  2021年   20篇
  2020年   51篇
  2019年   55篇
  2018年   67篇
  2017年   68篇
  2016年   61篇
  2015年   31篇
  2014年   64篇
  2013年   260篇
  2012年   103篇
  2011年   84篇
  2010年   66篇
  2009年   72篇
  2008年   104篇
  2007年   111篇
  2006年   88篇
  2005年   85篇
  2004年   78篇
  2003年   82篇
  2002年   64篇
  2001年   37篇
  2000年   34篇
  1999年   49篇
  1998年   47篇
  1997年   36篇
  1996年   30篇
  1995年   24篇
  1994年   21篇
  1993年   25篇
  1992年   27篇
  1991年   26篇
  1990年   32篇
  1989年   23篇
  1988年   22篇
  1987年   24篇
  1986年   25篇
  1985年   23篇
  1984年   20篇
  1983年   24篇
  1982年   20篇
  1981年   20篇
  1980年   21篇
  1979年   21篇
  1978年   17篇
  1977年   12篇
  1976年   13篇
  1975年   13篇
  1974年   19篇
  1973年   20篇
  1972年   12篇
排序方式: 共有2430条查询结果,搜索用时 6 毫秒
891.
In adolescence, children become increasingly independent and autonomous, and spend more time in neighborhood settings away from home. During mid-to-late adolescence, youth often become more critical about the place they live. Their attachment to home and even community may decrease as they explore and develop new attachments to other specific places. The aim of this study is to understand how 15-year-old students from 13 countries perceive their local neighborhood area (place attachment, social capital and safety), and how these different community cognitions are interrelated. We hypothesize that their place attachment predicts safety, and that the relationship is mediated in part by social capital. Result show that, despite cross-cultural differences in neighborhood perceptions, the proposed theoretical model fits robustly across all 13 countries.  相似文献   
892.
893.
894.
We evaluated rates of automatically reinforced stereotypy and item engagement for 2 children with autism under multiple and chained schedules in a multielement design. Each schedule included components during which stereotypy was blocked (S–) or allowed (S+), and we used colored cards as schedule‐correlated stimuli. We report rates of stereotypy and item engagement during S– and S+ components, as well as the percentage of component time that elapsed before the first instances of stereotypy and item engagement. We observed less stereotypy and more consistent item engagement during chained‐schedule sessions, and stimulus control of stereotypy and item engagement was established with the chained schedule. A subsequent concurrent‐chains analysis revealed participant preference for the chained schedule. These results highlight the importance of contingent access to stereotypy when therapists attempt to gain stimulus control of stereotypy and increase functional item engagement.  相似文献   
895.
896.
897.
898.
Philosophical Studies -  相似文献   
899.
900.
Abstract— When ratings of judged similarity or frequencies of stimulus identification are averaged across subjects, the psychological structure of the data is fundamentally changed. Regardless of the structure of the individual-subject data, the averaged similarity data will likely be well fit by a standard multidimensional scaling model, and the averaged identification data will likely be well fit by the similarity-choice model. In fact, both models often provide excellent fits to averaged data, even if they fail to fit the data of each individual subject. Thus, a good fit of either model to averaged data cannot be taken as evidence that the model describes the psychological structure that characterizes individual subjects. We hypothesize that these effects are due to the increased symmetry that is a mathematical consequence of the averaging operation.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号