全文获取类型
收费全文 | 2249篇 |
免费 | 18篇 |
专业分类
2267篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 12篇 |
2023年 | 11篇 |
2021年 | 16篇 |
2020年 | 43篇 |
2019年 | 45篇 |
2018年 | 65篇 |
2017年 | 51篇 |
2016年 | 48篇 |
2015年 | 32篇 |
2014年 | 55篇 |
2013年 | 258篇 |
2012年 | 101篇 |
2011年 | 87篇 |
2010年 | 54篇 |
2009年 | 67篇 |
2008年 | 108篇 |
2007年 | 114篇 |
2006年 | 94篇 |
2005年 | 77篇 |
2004年 | 76篇 |
2003年 | 77篇 |
2002年 | 60篇 |
2001年 | 50篇 |
2000年 | 36篇 |
1999年 | 43篇 |
1998年 | 39篇 |
1997年 | 31篇 |
1996年 | 27篇 |
1995年 | 18篇 |
1994年 | 18篇 |
1993年 | 24篇 |
1992年 | 18篇 |
1991年 | 22篇 |
1990年 | 28篇 |
1989年 | 26篇 |
1988年 | 20篇 |
1987年 | 19篇 |
1986年 | 22篇 |
1985年 | 22篇 |
1984年 | 19篇 |
1983年 | 15篇 |
1982年 | 18篇 |
1981年 | 22篇 |
1980年 | 16篇 |
1979年 | 20篇 |
1978年 | 13篇 |
1977年 | 11篇 |
1976年 | 12篇 |
1975年 | 20篇 |
1973年 | 12篇 |
排序方式: 共有2267条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
161.
Gregory PT Gregory LA 《Journal of comparative psychology (Washington, D.C. : 1983)》2006,120(3):262-268
A common response of animals to physical restraint is tonic immobility. The authors observed the behavior of garter snakes, Thamnophis elegans, in the field to determine the frequency of immobility as a response to handling. Most snakes fled after release, but the remainder remained immobile, sometimes on their backs (supination), for up to 10 min. Immobility was seen most often in pregnant snakes, which also were more likely than other snakes not to have moved before capture. Failure to move, either before or after capture, might be a consequence of the limited locomotory ability of gravid snakes. However, the lack of observations of interactions between snakes and their natural predators impedes researchers' understanding of the antipredator value, if any, of tonic immobility. 相似文献
162.
Gregory C.R Yates 《Journal of experimental child psychology》1974,18(2):333-339
Voluntary delay of gratification in 9-year-old children was assessed 4 weeks prior to, immediately after, and 4 weeks subsequent to exposure to one of four televized films. The modeling group observed an adult female model exemplify high-delay behavior. The persuasion group observed the model verbalize good reasons for waiting. The persuasion-modeling group observed the model exemplify high-delay behavior and verbalize reasons for waiting. Subjects in each of the treatment groups emitted more delay choices on the posttest than control group subjects who observed the model exhibit neutral behavior. The greatest magnitude of change occurred when modeling and persuasive cues were combined. Treatment effects were still in evidence for the modeling and the persuasion-modeling groups after a period of 4 weeks. 相似文献
163.
Much research on emotional facial expression employs posed expressions and expressive subjects. To test the generalizability of this research to more spontaneous expressions of both expressive and nonexpressive posers, subjects engaged in happy, sad, angry, and neutral imagery, and voluntarily posed happy, sad, and angry facial expressions while facial muscle activity (brow, cheek, and mouth regions) and autonomic activity (skin resistance and heart period) were recorded. Subjects were classified as expressive or nonexpressive on the basis of the intensity of their posed expressions. The posed and imagery-induced expressions were similar, but not identical. Brow activity present in the imagery-induced sad expressions was weak or absent in the posed ones. Both nonexpressive and expressive subjects demonstrated similar heart rate acceleration during emotional imagery and demonstrated similar posed and imagery-induced happy expressions, but nonexpressive subjects showed little facial activity during both their posed and imagery-induced sad and angry expressions. The implications of these findings are discussed. 相似文献
164.
165.
When one half of a randomly contoured pattern is displaced in one of four directions between successive exposures, Ss are capable of discriminating the direction of the displacement. Accuracy of discriminating decreases as a function of the relative displacement within the pattern, independent of the visual angle subtended by the pattern. Supplementary data suggest that Ss are unrealistically confident in the accuracy of their discrimination. The results demonstrate that identification of specific contours is not necessary for the discrimination of motion and suggest that some type of correlational process is employed by the visual system in dealing with spatially and temporally displaced patterns. 相似文献
166.
Male (N = 90) and female(N = 90) Ss were shown either 12 humorous or 12 serious TV commercials and were asked to rate each commercial on six 7-point personal feelings scales as well as on a 6-point evaluative (like-dislike) scale. The Ss were then shown the purported evaluative responses of a same-sex stranger which were .17, .50, or .83 similar to their own. Analysis of the personal feelings scales indicated expected differences (p < .001) in affect associated with the two sets of commercials. Females who viewed the serious commercials evidenced greater attraction toward the stranger than did females who saw the humorous commercials (p < .05). The similarity attraction function for males in the humorous condition had a greater slope and a lower y intercept than the similarity attraction function for males in the serious condition. A factor analysis of female affective responses to five socially relevant commercials in the serious set suggested the operation of two affective dimensions, one of which appeared to reflect feelings of social concern. 相似文献
167.
W.Curtis Banks Kenneth R Stitt Harriet Ann Curtis Gregory V McQuater 《Journal of experimental social psychology》1977,13(5):452-463
Two experiments were conducted to investigate the effects of positive and negative reinforcements upon black college students. In the first experiment subjects received either positive or negative reinforcement from a black or a white Evaluator. Behavioral change (compliance) was greater in response to negative reinforcement from a black Evaluator than in response to negative reinforcement from a white Evaluator, while there were no divergent effects of Evaluator's Race upon compliance with positive reinforcement. The same pattern obtained in subjects' self-evaluations. The second experiment offered a test of the hypothesis that the objectivity of black Evaluators was perceived as greater than that of white Evaluators, and that a manipulation aimed at equating the Evaluators on objectivity would attenuate the effects of Evaluator's Race upon compliance and self-evaluation in response to negative reinforcement. These hypotheses were generally supported by the results. 相似文献
168.
169.
John L. Bell 《Journal of Philosophical Logic》2000,29(6):585-601
In this paper the view is developed that classes should not be understood as individuals, but, rather, as classes as many of individuals. To correlate classes with individuals labelling and colabelling functions are introduced and sets identified with a certain subdomain of the classes on which the labelling and colabelling functions are mutually inverse. A minimal axiomatization of the resulting system is formulated and some of its extensions are related to various systems of set theory, including nonwellfounded set theories. 相似文献
170.
Left-handers with an inverted handwriting posture have been found by Levy and Reid to have relatively bilateral representation of tachistoscopically tested verbal and spatial functions. On the assumption that such bilateral representation is inefficient, it was hypothesized that left-inverted subjects (N = 20) would score lower than left- and right-handers with normal handwriting postures (Ns = 64, 66 respectively) on spatial reasoning. Results were consistent with the hypothesis. Possible alternative explanations and directions for future research are outlined. 相似文献