首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   22319篇
  免费   89篇
  2023年   13篇
  2022年   9篇
  2021年   19篇
  2020年   39篇
  2019年   46篇
  2018年   3520篇
  2017年   2851篇
  2016年   2279篇
  2015年   214篇
  2014年   121篇
  2013年   297篇
  2012年   650篇
  2011年   2447篇
  2010年   2551篇
  2009年   1527篇
  2008年   1782篇
  2007年   2271篇
  2006年   113篇
  2005年   294篇
  2004年   241篇
  2003年   186篇
  2002年   118篇
  2001年   67篇
  2000年   86篇
  1999年   62篇
  1998年   58篇
  1997年   48篇
  1996年   30篇
  1995年   20篇
  1994年   21篇
  1993年   22篇
  1992年   16篇
  1991年   24篇
  1990年   38篇
  1989年   29篇
  1988年   22篇
  1987年   17篇
  1986年   19篇
  1985年   20篇
  1984年   17篇
  1983年   16篇
  1982年   23篇
  1981年   23篇
  1980年   19篇
  1979年   20篇
  1978年   13篇
  1977年   9篇
  1975年   9篇
  1974年   13篇
  1973年   10篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 31 毫秒
181.
182.
Many researchers have proposed that, for the purpose of recognition, human vision parses shapes into component parts. Precisely how is not yet known. The minima rule for silhouettes (Hoffman & Richards, 1984) defines boundary points at which to parse but does not tell how to use these points to cut silhouettes and, therefore, does not tell what the parts are. In this paper, we propose the short-cut rule, which states that, other things being equal, human vision prefers to use the shortest possible cuts to parse silhouettes. We motivate this rule, and the well-known Petter’s rule for modal completion, by the principle of transversality. We present five psychophysical experiments that test the short-cut rule, show that it successfully predicts part cuts that connect boundary points given by the minima rule, and show that it can also create new boundary points.  相似文献   
183.
In two experiments, we examined whether voluntary and reflexive saccades shared a common fixation disengagement mechanism, Participants were required to perform a variety of tasks, each requiring a different level of information processing of the display prior to execution of the saccade. In Experiment 1, participants executed either a prosaccade or an antisaccade upon detecting a stimulus array. In Experiment 2, participants executed a prosaccade to a stimulus array only if the array contained a target item. The target could be a line (easy search) or a digit (difficult search). The critical manipulation in both experiments was the relative timing between the removal of the fixation stimulus and the onset of the stimulus array. In both experiments, it was found that saccadic latencies were shortest when the fixation stimulus was removed before the onset of the stimulus array—a gap effect. It was concluded that reflexive and voluntary saccades share a common fixation disengagement mechanism that is largely independent of higher level cognitive processes.  相似文献   
184.
185.
186.
Murphy GL  Ross BH 《Memory & cognition》1999,27(6):1024-1041
One of the main functions of categories is to allow inferences about new objects. However, most objects are cross-classified, and it is not known whether and how people combine information from these different categories in making inferences. In six experiments, food categories, which are strongly crossclassified (e.g., a bagel is both a bread and a breakfast food), were studied. For each food, the subjects were told fictitious facts (e.g., 75% of breads are subject to spoilage from Aspergillus molds) about two of the categories to which it belonged and then were asked to make an inference about the food (e.g., how likely is a bagel to be subject to spoilage from Aspergillus molds?). We found no more use of multiple categories in these cases of cross-classification than in ambiguous classification, in which it is uncertain to which category an item belongs. However, some procedural manipulations did markedly increase the use of both categories in inferences, primarily those that focused the subjects’ attention on the critical feature in both categories.  相似文献   
187.
188.
189.
190.
This article considers the meaning and significance of authority, and its relevance to the transference process, within the framework of psychotherapy in the orthodox Jewish (Haredi) community in Israel. In this community, deeply-rooted habits of obedience to the commandments of the Torah and the authority of the Rabbi are integral to maintaining an orthodox way of life. Clinical vignettes with Haredi patients are presented to illustrate the complexities that arise when both patient and therapist belong to the orthodox community, and highlight the authority-related issues that are central to the therapy. This combination of factors requires a sensitive and finely-tuned approach which will enable the therapist to maintain the treatment framework while still accommodating the orthodox way of life.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号