全文获取类型
收费全文 | 29328篇 |
免费 | 565篇 |
国内免费 | 2篇 |
专业分类
29895篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 353篇 |
2019年 | 484篇 |
2018年 | 595篇 |
2017年 | 535篇 |
2016年 | 658篇 |
2015年 | 431篇 |
2014年 | 595篇 |
2013年 | 2628篇 |
2012年 | 1007篇 |
2011年 | 1045篇 |
2010年 | 713篇 |
2009年 | 730篇 |
2008年 | 1001篇 |
2007年 | 1011篇 |
2006年 | 923篇 |
2005年 | 778篇 |
2004年 | 841篇 |
2003年 | 765篇 |
2002年 | 746篇 |
2001年 | 654篇 |
2000年 | 720篇 |
1999年 | 540篇 |
1998年 | 412篇 |
1997年 | 324篇 |
1996年 | 284篇 |
1995年 | 320篇 |
1994年 | 303篇 |
1993年 | 278篇 |
1992年 | 414篇 |
1991年 | 414篇 |
1990年 | 413篇 |
1989年 | 387篇 |
1988年 | 390篇 |
1987年 | 352篇 |
1986年 | 339篇 |
1985年 | 369篇 |
1984年 | 319篇 |
1983年 | 307篇 |
1982年 | 275篇 |
1981年 | 265篇 |
1980年 | 266篇 |
1979年 | 360篇 |
1978年 | 330篇 |
1975年 | 285篇 |
1974年 | 323篇 |
1973年 | 326篇 |
1972年 | 269篇 |
1971年 | 254篇 |
1968年 | 268篇 |
1967年 | 258篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
841.
Developmental research is used to transform existing knowledge into applied programs. A social research and development (R&D) method, developed by Rothman for the human service professions, is compared with the method used to develop the Suicide Prevention Training Programs in Alberta, Canada. A retrospective review compares the development of these programs, which have since been implemented both nationally and internationally, with Rothman's model; discusses social R&D as a transformation methodology; and draws conclusions about the probable success of using developmental research methods in the human services. The review finds that the Alberta method closely paralleled the phases of Rothman's model. Results show that the transformation of knowledge about suicide into widely disseminated suicide prevention training programs can be attributed to the application of social R&D principles. The study concludes that the use of developmental research methods to establish human service programs should increase the probability of their success. 相似文献
842.
MIRANDA OLFF GUIDO L. R. GODAERT JOS F. BROSSCHOT KATIE E. WEISS HOLGER URSIN 《Scandinavian journal of psychology》1990,31(2):89-98
Only few attempts have been made to compare different methods aiming at quantifying defensive strategies. In this study the tachistoscopic Defence Mechanism Test (Kragh, 1960b and 1985) is compared to paper-and-pencil tests of defence. There were virtually no correlations between the DMT and the other measures of defence. This may indicate that the concept of defence is a highly complex one. It is suggested that the Defence Mechanism Test may be measuring primary defence while defence questionnaires may be assessing more secondary forms of defence. The two questionnaires used for assessing defence correlated significantly, while a scale for social desirability showed no correlation with DMT or the two defence questionnaires. 相似文献
843.
An experiment was conducted to determine how visual attention is allocated in response to pairwise combinations of location, color, and size cues. Subjects viewed displays of eight letters arranged into an 'X' pattern emanating from a central fixation point. The letters varied on the dimensions of location (foveal or parafoveal positions), size (large or small), and color (red or blue). Seven of the letters were 'X's and the remaining, critical letter was either an 'R' or an 'L'. Subjects were given advance cues about characteristics of the critical letter on two of the three dimensions (location size, and color). The cues varied in their validity such that on any trial either both dimensions were validly cued, one dimension was validly cued while the other was invalidly cued, or both cues were invalid. Subjects were instructed to try to use the cues to find the critical letter, and response times to identify it as an 'R' or an 'L' served as the main dependent measure. Two models were evaluated against the data; in one, location cues are viewed as having distinct effects on an early stage of attention whereas color and size cues operate only in a later, selection stage. In the other model, all cues are used in a similar way to select display items for serial, focal attention. In general, the serial processing model provides a more complete account of the data. 相似文献
844.
J T Todd F D Reichel 《Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance》1990,16(3):665-674
This research was designed to examine how human observers are able to perceive the 3-dimensional structure of smoothly curved surfaces from projected patterns of surface contours. Displays were generated by using a method of double projection that made it possible to cover a surface with a wide range of contour patterns of varying geometric structure and to eliminate systematic variations of image shading. The compelling impression of 3-dimensional form from these patterns provides strong evidence that the ability of observers to perceptually interpret surface contours is considerably less restrictive than would be reasonable to expect on the basis of existing computational models. Results suggest that the perceptual analysis of surface contours is able to exploit statistical regularities of contour structure over appropriately large regions of visual space. 相似文献
845.
846.
J P Green S J Lynn J R Weekes B W Carlson J Brentar L Latham R Kurzhals 《Journal of abnormal psychology》1990,99(1):16-21
The responses given by highly hypnotizable, hypnotic subjects and those of unhypnotizable subjects who simulated hypnosis to questions of the type, "Do you mind telling me your name?" and "Do you mind standing up?" were contrasted. The purpose was to examine Erickson's (1980) assertion that literalism (answering "yes" or "no" verbally or nonverbally without any cognitive elaboration) is a marker of hypnotic "trance." Simulators exhibited a greater rate of literalism than hypnotic "virtuosos" (i.e., extreme scorers on both group and individual hypnotizability measures). Hypnotized subjects and nonhypnotized subjects approached in the campus library responded comparably. Because less than a third of hypnotic virtuosos responded literally, our results strongly refuted Erickson's assertion that literalism is a cognitive feature of hypnosis. 相似文献
847.
T W Smith J D Sanders J F Alexander 《Journal of personality and social psychology》1990,58(4):699-708
The Cook and Medley (1954) Hostility (Ho) scale has been used in several important studies evaluating potential health consequences of hostility. A relative lack of compelling information about the construct validity of the Ho scale, however, has raised concerns about the appropriate interpretation of previous research. In this study, 60 married couples discussed a low conflict topic, a high conflict topic, and then a second low conflict topic. High Ho men responded to the high conflict topic with significant increases in self-reported anger and anxiety and overt hostile behavior, but low Ho men did not. Furthermore, compared to low Ho men, high Ho men blamed their wives more for their usual disagreements on the high conflict topic and saw their disagreement-engendering behavior as more intentional. Among women, Ho scores were weakly related only to overt hostile behavior. Finally, couples consisting of two low Ho persons displayed a uniquely agreeable interactional style. 相似文献
848.
This article presents the development of a 60-item self-report known as the Four Systems Anxiety Questionnaire (FSAQ). The FSAQ assesses anxiety on four components: somatic, cognitive, behavioral, and feeling. The FSAQ was given to 218 first-year university students, 54 anxiety patients attending clinical psychology departments, and 14 agoraphobic patients pre- and postpsychological treatment. Results reveal satisfactory reliability and validity levels. In addition, we proved that the FSAQ is sensitive to change following psychological treatment. 相似文献
849.
J F Chused 《Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association》1990,38(3):679-704
An adolescent patient's action during analysis reflects both neurotic conflicts and the developmentally determined task of establishing an integrated self-representation. Concern for the consequences of the action often provokes the analyst to respond, covertly, with interventions intended to change the action through influence rather than understanding. This can lead to a distortion of the analytic process which, in itself, may be an enactment of the developmental conflict. Examination of such interventions reveals a lack of analytic neutrality and an unconscious participation in the patient's neurotic and developmental conflicts. Clinical material from the analyses of two fourteen-year-old girls and a sixteen-year-old boy is presented to illustrate and support this hypothesis. 相似文献
850.
D H Peterzell G P Sinclair A F Healy L E Bourne 《The American journal of psychology》1990,103(3):299-315
The size of the perceptual unit used in reading was addressed using the predesignated target paradigm. Sixteen subjects viewed the following stimuli in random order: the words tee, the, tie, and toe; the nonwords eet, eht, eit, and eot; and the letters e, h, i, and o. Subjects fixated on the location of the center letter and identified the letter as e, h, i, or o, alternatives which were known to them at the onset. A word superiority effect was obtained for the common word the but not for the less common words tee, tie, and toe. The word superiority effect was attributable to bias rather than discriminability: Subjects exhibited a bias to perceive the words in this experiment as the (i.e., there was a bias to perceive h in the t e stimulus presentations). These results suggest that the common word the is processed in reading units that are larger than the letter, and that the system is biased to perceive common rather than uncommon words in data-limited conditions. 相似文献