首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   1013篇
  免费   56篇
  1069篇
  2023年   13篇
  2021年   8篇
  2020年   20篇
  2019年   14篇
  2018年   27篇
  2017年   23篇
  2016年   39篇
  2015年   28篇
  2014年   26篇
  2013年   90篇
  2012年   48篇
  2011年   40篇
  2010年   28篇
  2009年   24篇
  2008年   32篇
  2007年   45篇
  2006年   44篇
  2005年   32篇
  2004年   33篇
  2003年   33篇
  2002年   33篇
  2001年   18篇
  2000年   13篇
  1999年   15篇
  1998年   9篇
  1997年   10篇
  1996年   14篇
  1994年   7篇
  1993年   14篇
  1992年   18篇
  1991年   26篇
  1990年   10篇
  1989年   13篇
  1988年   9篇
  1985年   8篇
  1983年   8篇
  1982年   8篇
  1981年   7篇
  1980年   8篇
  1978年   17篇
  1977年   15篇
  1976年   8篇
  1975年   9篇
  1974年   18篇
  1971年   9篇
  1970年   7篇
  1969年   6篇
  1968年   10篇
  1967年   8篇
  1956年   6篇
排序方式: 共有1069条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
101.
102.
103.
104.
105.
106.
107.
108.
Behavioral addictions, such as pathological gambling, kleptomania, pyromania, compulsive buying, and compulsive sexual behavior, represent significant public health concerns and are associated with high rates of psychiatric comorbidity and mortality. Although research into the biology of these behaviors is still in the early stages, recent advances in the understanding of motivation, reward, and addiction have provided insight into the possible pathophysiology of these disorders. Biochemical, functional neuroimaging, genetic studies, and treatment research have suggested a strong neurobiological link between behavioral addictions and substance use disorders. Given the substantial co-occurrence of these groups of disorders, improved understanding of their relationship has important implications not only for further understanding the neurobiology of both categories of disorders but also for improving prevention and treatment strategies.  相似文献   
109.
110.
In the first three experiments, subjects felt solid geometrical forms and matched raised-line pictures to the objects. Performance was best in experiment 1 for top views, with shorter response latencies than for side views, front views, or 3-D views with foreshortening. In a second experiment with blind participants, matching accuracy was not significantly affected by prior visual experience, but speed advantages were found for top views, with 3-D views also yielding better matching accuracy than side views. There were no performance advantages for pictures of objects with a constant cross section in the vertical axis. The early-blind participants had lower performance for side and frontal views. The objects were rotated to oblique orientations in experiment 3. Early-blind subjects performed worse than the other subjects given object rotation. Visual experience with pictures of objects at many angles could facilitate identification at oblique orientations. In experiment 5 with blindfolded sighted subjects, tangible pictures were used as targets and as choices. The results yielded superior overall performance for 3-D views (mean, M = 74% correct) and much lower matching accuracy for top views as targets (M = 58% correct). Performance was highest when the target and matching viewpoint were identical, but 3-D views (M = 96% correct) were still far better than top views. The accuracy advantage of the top views also disappeared when more complex objects were tested in experiment 6. Alternative theoretical implications of the results are discussed.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号