全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1087篇 |
免费 | 14篇 |
专业分类
1101篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 6篇 |
2021年 | 6篇 |
2020年 | 14篇 |
2019年 | 16篇 |
2018年 | 28篇 |
2017年 | 20篇 |
2016年 | 29篇 |
2015年 | 20篇 |
2014年 | 27篇 |
2013年 | 147篇 |
2012年 | 31篇 |
2011年 | 34篇 |
2010年 | 23篇 |
2009年 | 31篇 |
2008年 | 32篇 |
2007年 | 37篇 |
2006年 | 53篇 |
2005年 | 46篇 |
2004年 | 21篇 |
2003年 | 31篇 |
2002年 | 28篇 |
2001年 | 20篇 |
2000年 | 21篇 |
1999年 | 28篇 |
1998年 | 24篇 |
1997年 | 17篇 |
1996年 | 13篇 |
1995年 | 16篇 |
1994年 | 12篇 |
1993年 | 17篇 |
1992年 | 13篇 |
1991年 | 23篇 |
1990年 | 10篇 |
1989年 | 15篇 |
1988年 | 6篇 |
1987年 | 6篇 |
1986年 | 10篇 |
1985年 | 13篇 |
1984年 | 16篇 |
1983年 | 10篇 |
1982年 | 16篇 |
1981年 | 14篇 |
1979年 | 12篇 |
1978年 | 8篇 |
1977年 | 6篇 |
1976年 | 8篇 |
1975年 | 6篇 |
1974年 | 8篇 |
1968年 | 8篇 |
1966年 | 6篇 |
排序方式: 共有1101条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
941.
Previous research has shown that we recognize faces similar in age to ourselves better than older or younger faces (e.g.,
Wright & Stroud, 2002). This study investigated whether this own-age bias could be explained by the contact hypothesis used to account for the own-race bias (see Meissner & Brigham, 2001). If the own-age bias stems from increased exposure to
people of our own age, it should be reduced or absent in those with higher exposure to other age groups. Participants were
asked to remember facial photographs of 8- to 11- and 20- to 25-year-olds. Undergraduates were faster and more accurate at
recognizing faces of their own age. However, trainee teachers showed no such own-age bias; they recognized the children’s
faces more quickly than own-age faces and with comparable accuracy. These results support a contact-based explanation of the
own-age bias. 相似文献
942.
Juliana Onwumere Elizabeth Kuipers Catherine Gamble Suzanne Jolley Ben Smith Rebecca Rollinson Craig Steel David Fowler Paul Bebbington Graham Dunn Daniel Freeman Philippa Garety 《Journal of Family Therapy》2009,31(3):270-283
The efficacy of family interventions in psychosis is well documented. UK and USA schizophrenia treatment guidelines advocate the practice of family interventions within routine clinical services. However, less attention has been paid to the study of treatment fidelity and the tools used in its assessment. This study reports the inter-rater reliability of a new scale: Family Intervention in Psychosis-Adherence Scale (FIPAS). This measure is designed to assess therapist adherence to the Kuipers et al . (2002) family intervention in psychosis treatment manual. Reliability ratings were based on a sample of thirteen audiotapes drawn from a randomized controlled trial of family intervention. The results indicated that the majority of items of the FIPAS had acceptable levels of inter-rater reliability. The findings are discussed in terms of their implications for the training and monitoring of the effectiveness of practitioners for family interventions in psychosis. 相似文献
943.
People experience autonomy when they perceive their behaviour to be volitional rather than driven by external controls. Previous
research has studied autonomy in relationships at a general level, focusing on people’s motivations to maintain their romantic
relationships, as measured by the Couple Motivation Questionnaire (CMQ; Blais et al., J Personal Soc Psychol 59:1021–1031,
1990). To supplement the CMQ, we developed the Motivations for Relational Activities (MRA) scale, which assesses the extent to
which people feel autonomous and controlled in a variety of specific relational activities. The purpose of this study is to
examine the unique contributions of general motivations to maintain a relationship (CMQ) and motivations toward specific relational
activities (MRA) in the prediction of relationship well-being. Results showed that the MRA and CMQ both independently and
significantly contributed to the prediction of relationship well-being (i.e., commitment, intimacy, satisfaction, and vitality
within the relationship) and were differentiated by their associations to dimensions of personality and attachment.
相似文献
Graham S. GaineEmail: |
944.
Graham Priest 《Australasian journal of philosophy》2013,91(4):789-795
In ‘Definability and the Structure of Logical Paradoxes’ (Australasian Journal of Philosophy, this issue) Haixia Zhong takes issue with an account of the paradoxes of self-reference to be found in Beyond the Limits of Thought [Priest 1995. The point of this note is to explain why the critique does not succeed. The criterion for distinguishing between the set-theoretic and the semantic paradoxes offered does not get the division right; the semantic paradoxes are not given a uniform solution; no reason is provided as to why the naïve denotation relation is ‘indefinite’ (other than that its definiteness leads to contradiction); and the account of the denotation relation given clearly misses the mark, even by consistent standards. 相似文献
945.
946.
Susan A. Graham Laura L. Namy Kristinn Meagher 《Journal of experimental child psychology》2010,107(3):280-290
We examined the role of the comparison process and shared names on preschoolers’ categorization of novel objects. In our studies, 4-year-olds were presented with novel object sets consisting of either one or two standards and two test objects: a shape match and a texture match. When children were presented with one standard, they extended the category based on shape regardless of whether the objects were named. When children were presented with two standards that shared the same texture and the objects were named with the same noun, they extended the category based on texture. The opportunity to compare two standards, in the absence of shared names, led to an attenuation of the effect of shape. These findings demonstrate that comparison plays a critical role in the categorization of novel objects and that shared names enhance this process. 相似文献
947.
Carol Graham 《Applied research in quality of life》2011,6(3):219-239
Easterlin’s famous paradox questioned standard economic assumptions about a fundamental relationship in economics: that between
happiness and income. In recent years there has been renewed debate about the paradox. In this essay, I highlight some of
the methodological issues and challenges underlying that debate. I focus on the sensitivity of the results to the method selected,
the choice of micro or macro data, and the way that happiness questions are defined and framed, all of which result in divergent
conclusions. I also note the mediating role of the pace and nature of economic growth, institutional frameworks, and inequality.
What is most notable is the remarkable consistency in the determinants of individual happiness – including income – within countries of diverse income levels and, at the same time, how happiness is affected by cross-country differences that are related to average per-capita income levels, such as political freedom and public goods. Income clearly
plays a role in determining both individual and country level happiness. Still, assessing its role relative to other more
difficult to measure factors as countries develop in new ways and at different rates will remain a challenge for the foreseeable
future. 相似文献
948.
Clarke GS 《Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association》2011,59(5):939-959
It is suggested here that Ian Suttie influenced W. R. D. Fairbairn directly through a 1939 reprint of his The Origins of Love and Hate, a heavily underlined copy of which was found by the author in Fairbairn's library in the University of Edinburgh Library Special Collections in October 2009. Underlined sections of the book are compared with significant aspects of Fairbairn's post-1940 theorizing, and the similarities are argued to be due to Fairbairn's adopting many of the underlying attitudes and ideas that Suttie developed. This leaves Fairbairn's structural theory, which he began to develop as a rational reconstruction of Freud's structural theory as early as 1927, dependent in part on such theory but independent of direct influence by Suttie. It is argued that Suttie's importance vis-à-vis British object relations thinking needs to be reassessed. 相似文献
949.
Peter A. Graham 《Philosophy and phenomenological research》2014,88(2):388-409
In this paper I sketch an account of moral blame and blameworthiness. I begin by clarifying what I take blame to be and explaining how blameworthiness is to be analyzed in terms of it. I then consider different accounts of the conditions of blameworthiness and, in the end, settle on one according to which a person is blameworthy for φ‐ing just in case, in φ‐ing, she violates one of a particular class of moral requirements governing the attitudes we bear, and our mental orientation, toward people and other objects of significant moral worth. These requirements embody the moral stricture that we accord to these others a sufficient level of respect, one that their moral worth demands. This is a familiar theme which has its roots in P. F. Strawson’s pioneering views on moral responsibility. My development of it leads me to the conclusion that acting wrongly is not a condition of blameworthiness: violating a moral requirement to perform, or refrain from performing, an action is neither necessary nor sufficient for being blameworthy. All we are ever blameworthy for, I will argue, are certain aspects of our mental bearing toward others. We can be said to be blameworthy for our actions only derivatively, in the sense that those actions are the natural manifestations of the things for which we are strictly speaking blameworthy. 相似文献
950.
Carlo O. C. Veltri John R. Graham Martin Sellbom Yossef S. Ben-Porath Johnathan D. Forbey Carol O'Connell 《Journal of personality assessment》2013,95(3):288-300
The purpose of this study was to expand the empirical basis for interpretation of the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory–Adolescent (MMPI–A; Butcher et al., 1992). Participants were 157 boys from a forensic setting and 197 girls from an acute psychiatric inpatient setting. Criterion variables were identified from sources such as psychiatrist report, parent report, and psychosocial history. Results generally support the construct validity of MMPI–A scales. Scales measuring internalizing problems were more highly correlated with criterion measures of internalizing behaviors than measures of externalizing behaviors, whereas scales measuring externalizing problems were more highly correlated with externalizing variables than with internalizing criteria. Implications of this study include an expanded empirical foundation for interpretation of the MMPI–A, greater understanding of the constructs it measures, and evidence supporting the generalizability of these constructs across settings. 相似文献