首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   498篇
  免费   55篇
  国内免费   61篇
  2024年   4篇
  2023年   9篇
  2022年   16篇
  2021年   13篇
  2020年   23篇
  2019年   20篇
  2018年   22篇
  2017年   29篇
  2016年   21篇
  2015年   17篇
  2014年   33篇
  2013年   39篇
  2012年   26篇
  2011年   34篇
  2010年   25篇
  2009年   24篇
  2008年   36篇
  2007年   33篇
  2006年   32篇
  2005年   20篇
  2004年   17篇
  2003年   11篇
  2002年   11篇
  2001年   5篇
  2000年   13篇
  1999年   11篇
  1998年   4篇
  1997年   5篇
  1996年   8篇
  1995年   6篇
  1994年   6篇
  1993年   5篇
  1992年   4篇
  1991年   2篇
  1990年   3篇
  1988年   3篇
  1986年   3篇
  1984年   2篇
  1983年   2篇
  1982年   1篇
  1981年   1篇
  1978年   2篇
  1977年   1篇
  1976年   1篇
  1975年   1篇
  1974年   3篇
  1970年   1篇
  1966年   1篇
  1950年   1篇
  1933年   1篇
排序方式: 共有614条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
151.
According to behavioral momentum theory (Nevin & Grace, 2000a), preference in concurrent chains and resistance to change in multiple schedules are independent measures of a common construct representing reinforcement history. Here I review the original studies on preference and resistance to change in which reinforcement variables were manipulated parametrically, conducted by Nevin, Grace and colleagues between 1997 and 2002, as well as more recent research. The cumulative decision model proposed by Grace and colleagues for concurrent chains is shown to provide a good account of both preference and resistance to change, and is able to predict the increased sensitivity to reinforcer rate and magnitude observed with constant‐duration components. Residuals from fits of the cumulative decision model to preference and resistance to change data were positively correlated, supporting the prediction of behavioral momentum theory. Although some questions remain, the learning process assumed by the cumulative decision model, in which outcomes are compared against a criterion that represents the average outcome value in the current context, may provide a plausible model for the acquisition of differential resistance to change.  相似文献   
152.
In two experiments, experimentally naïve rats were trained in concurrent variable‐interval schedules in which the reinforcer ratios changed daily according to a pseudorandom binary sequence. In Experiment 1, relative response rates showed clear sensitivity to current‐session reinforcer ratios, but not to previous sessions' reinforcer ratios. Within sessions, sensitivity to the current session's reinforcement rates increased steadily, and by session end, response ratios approached matching to the current‐session reinforcer ratios. Across sessions, sensitivity to the current session's reinforcer ratio decreased with continued exposure to the pseudorandom binary sequence, contrary to expectations based on previous studies demonstrating learning sets. Using a second group of naïve rats, Experiment 2 replicated the main results from Experiment 1 and showed that although there were increases over sessions in both changeover rate and response rate during the changeover delay, neither could explain the accompanying reductions in sensitivity. We consider the role of reinforcement history, showing that our results can be simulated using two separate representations, one local and one nonlocal, but a more complex approach will be needed to bring together these results and other history effects such as learning sets and spontaneous recovery.  相似文献   
153.
姚润皋 《学海》2011,(3):5-12
善于总结经验是中国共产党的优良传统。改革开放以来中国共产党总结历史和现实经验的总体情况,可以从经验所涉及的时间跨度、概括内容、表述方式等方面来把握;这一时期党总结经验的基本特点是科学性、政治导向性、深刻性和动态性。坚持以科学思维方法和正确思想路线为指导,是总结经验具有科学性的必要条件;坚定不移高举中国特色社会主义伟大旗帜,是政治导向性的集中表现;揭示社会主义现代化建设客观规律、形成中国特色社会主义理论体系,是深刻性的生动体现;遵循人类认识发展规律,在总结经验中实现主客观之间具体、历史的统一,是动态性的内在根据。科学性是政治导向性、深刻性和动态性的基础,深刻性、动态性是科学性的要求和体现;科学性和政治导向性的结合,从根本上保证中国特色社会主义的正确方向,促进其蓬勃发展。  相似文献   
154.
吕耀军 《世界宗教研究》2011,(5):149-156,194
跨文化人权理论为当代伊斯兰人权哲学提供了新的思路。现代国家观念的发展和国家之间的互动是该理论产生影响的主因。穆斯林学者强调伊斯兰文化中,关于尊严、公正和平等的伦理精神是实现伊斯兰跨文化人权普遍性的基础。这就需要依据经典的原初精神和现代理念,对伊斯兰法做出新的解释。人类社会生活的共同需要、人性的共同趋向和人权的跨文化视角并不会削弱宗教的核心原则的观点,是穆斯林学者主张跨文化人权理论的依据。在此基础上,学者发展了"重叠共识"理论,结合伊斯兰国家人权情况,提出了实现跨文化人权理论的具体方式。  相似文献   
155.
陈瑶 《管子学刊》2011,(1):74-79
《左传》塑造了众多尚力崇勇的齐国勇士,他们注重于对个人功名的争取,具有英雄气概和冒险精神。长于单打独斗而怯于众斗,成为齐文化的重要特色。《左传》的战争叙事,对此作了生动地展现。  相似文献   
156.
马华维  杨柳  姚琦 《心理学探新》2011,31(2):186-191
随着组织联盟与合作的增多,组织间信任问题日益得到研究者关注。文章尝试从动、静态的角度对组织间信任的前因变量和结果变量进行梳理:单方组织层面的人口学特征及获得性特征,以及涉及双方互动层面的过去经历、目前状态及未来期望等因素会影响组织间信任的建立,降低双方合作的风险;而已经形成的组织间信任又会起到促进新知识的应用,提高联盟绩效及合作满意度的作用。今后的研究应关注组织信任的多层次分析、重视研究对象领域和地域的多样化以及互动(配对)和纵向数据的收集等。  相似文献   
157.
Psychosocial factors are increasingly recognized as risk indicators for coronary artery disease (CAD) prognosis and they are likely interrelated. The objective of this study is to simultaneously test the relationship between key psychosocial constructs as independent factor scores and recurrent events in CAD patients. There were 1268 CAD outpatients of 97 cardiologists surveyed at two points. Recurrent events or hospitalization in the intervening nine months were reported. Factor analysis of items from the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale, Perceived Stress Scale, the ENRICHD Social Support Inventory, and Hostile Attitudes Scale was performed to generate orthogonal factor scores. With adjustment for prognostic variables, logistic regression analysis was performed to examine the relationship between these factor scores and recurrent events. Factor analysis resulted in a six-factor solution: hostility, stress, anxiety, depressive symptoms, support, and resilience. Logistic regression revealed that functional status and anxiety, with a trend for depressive symptoms, were related to experiencing a recurrent event. In this simultaneous test of psychosocial constructs hypothesized to relate to cardiac prognosis, anxiety may be a particularly hazardous psychosocial factor. While replication is warranted, efforts to investigate the potential benefits of screening and to investigate treatments are needed.  相似文献   
158.
Recent theoretical and empirical developments in human category learning have differentiated an analytic, rule-based system of category learning from a nonanalytic system that integrates information across stimulus dimensions. In the present study, the researchers applied this theoretical distinction to pigeons' category learning. Pigeons learned to categorize stimuli varying in the tilt and width of their internal striping. The matched category problems had either a unidimensional (rule-based) or multidimensional (information-integration) solution. Whereas humans and nonhuman primates strongly dimensionalize these stimuli and learn rule-based tasks far more quickly than information-integration tasks, pigeons learned the two tasks equally quickly to the same accuracy level. Pigeons may represent a cognitive system in which the commitment to dimensional analysis and category rules was not strongly made. Their performance could suggest the character of the ancestral vertebrate categorization system from which that of primates emerged.  相似文献   
159.
The phenomenon of post-traumatic stress has been well documented in the literature as a lasting mental health condition associated with exposure to traumatic life events. The diagnosis and experience of human immunodeficiency virus (HIV) disease may be such a trauma. On the other hand, the phenomenon of post-traumatic growth (PTG) has been described, whereby people show positive mental health growth in the face of such trauma. This systematic review was set out to explore post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and PTG in people with HIV to monitor prevalence, measurement and efficacy of interventions to reduce stress and/or promote growth. Standardised review techniques were used to track reports on both PTSD and PTG. A total of 206 papers were retrieved from the PTSD and HIV searches, and 13 from the PTG and HIV searches. After reviewing the papers for inclusion according to adequacy and relevance criteria and to remove duplicates, 33 PTSD papers and three PTG were available for full coding. Prevalence of PTSD in HIV ranged from 5% to 74%, which were much greater than the 7-10% in the general population. Seven studies showed a relation between trauma and PTSD, while six showed a link between PTSD diagnosis and reduced antiretroviral treatment adherence. Women were more likely to be diagnosed with PTSD. Only three intervention reports were identified that fitted our inclusion criteria. All of these reported on psychological interventions for HIV+?individuals with trauma. The interventions utilised HIV education, training in coping techniques and support groups. Only coping interventions were shown to be effective. PTG was under researched but showed a promising avenue of study. There needs to be harmonised measurement and the evidence base would need strengthening in order to build on the understanding of the impact of PTSD and PTG over the course of HIV disease. There is good evidence to associate HIV diagnosis and experiences during the course of illness as traumatic. PTSD has been shown to be prevalent and there seems to be good evidence to incorporate standardised measures to track the course of the disorder. There is extremely limited evidence that interventions may affect the course of symptom experience. The evidence and insight into PTG show promise but is currently inadequate.  相似文献   
160.
Nevin JA  Grace RC 《The Behavioral and brain sciences》2000,23(1):73-90; discussion 90-130
In the metaphor of behavioral momentum, the rate of a free operant in the presence of a discriminative stimulus is analogous to the velocity of a moving body, and resistance to change measures an aspect of behavior that is analogous to its inertial mass. An extension of the metaphor suggests that preference measures an analog to the gravitational mass of that body. The independent functions relating resistance to change and preference to the conditions of reinforcement may be construed as convergent measures of a single construct, analogous to physical mass, that represents the effects of a history of exposure to the signaled conditions of reinforcement and that unifies the traditionally separate notions of the strength of learning and the value of incentives. Research guided by the momentum metaphor encompasses the effects of reinforcement on response rate, resistance to change, and preference and has implications for clinical interventions, drug addiction, and self-control. In addition, its principles can be seen as a modern, quantitative version of Thorndike's (1911) Law of Effect, providing a new perspective on some of the challenges to his postulation of strengthening by reinforcement.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号