全文获取类型
收费全文 | 6909篇 |
免费 | 372篇 |
国内免费 | 8篇 |
专业分类
7289篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 78篇 |
2022年 | 109篇 |
2021年 | 125篇 |
2020年 | 173篇 |
2019年 | 175篇 |
2018年 | 330篇 |
2017年 | 313篇 |
2016年 | 303篇 |
2015年 | 186篇 |
2014年 | 213篇 |
2013年 | 901篇 |
2012年 | 497篇 |
2011年 | 463篇 |
2010年 | 307篇 |
2009年 | 215篇 |
2008年 | 370篇 |
2007年 | 329篇 |
2006年 | 287篇 |
2005年 | 258篇 |
2004年 | 173篇 |
2003年 | 182篇 |
2002年 | 171篇 |
2001年 | 85篇 |
2000年 | 112篇 |
1999年 | 73篇 |
1998年 | 48篇 |
1997年 | 36篇 |
1996年 | 33篇 |
1995年 | 30篇 |
1994年 | 22篇 |
1993年 | 25篇 |
1992年 | 45篇 |
1991年 | 28篇 |
1990年 | 35篇 |
1989年 | 32篇 |
1988年 | 38篇 |
1987年 | 32篇 |
1985年 | 23篇 |
1984年 | 28篇 |
1983年 | 19篇 |
1982年 | 17篇 |
1979年 | 25篇 |
1978年 | 20篇 |
1976年 | 21篇 |
1973年 | 20篇 |
1972年 | 17篇 |
1971年 | 18篇 |
1970年 | 21篇 |
1968年 | 20篇 |
1967年 | 17篇 |
排序方式: 共有7289条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
41.
Stig Fhanér 《Psychometrika》1966,31(2):267-269
It is shown that certain correlation measures suggested by information theory are simple functions of the determinants of the correlation matrices involved, if multivariate normality is assumed. This illuminates and in a sense strengthens Rozeboom's point in [3], that information theoretical statistics have classical correlational counterparts. 相似文献
42.
43.
Rodolphe Gasché 《Argumentation》1990,4(1):85-100
In the Critique of Judgement, Kant, despite his strong condemnation of rhetoric, introduces the figure of hypotyposis at the very moment he sets out to tackle the philosophical problem of presentation as such. This study holds that this choice of the rhetorical term is not fortuitous. Its connotations of vivid illustration, synopsis, and moral grandeur serve Kant in arguing that, on a transcendental level, presentation secures the mind's life, unity, and self-affection. Although of rhetorical origin, hypotyposis is thus shown to link up with a specifically philosophical meaning of the term in the writings of Aristotle. 相似文献
44.
When an observer views a moving scene binocularly, both motion parallax and binocular disparity provide depth information. In Experiments lA-1C, we measured sensitivity to surface curvature when these depth cues were available either individually or simultaneously. When the depth cues yielded comparable sensitivity to surface curvature, we found that curvature detection was easier with the cues present simultaneously, rather than individually. For 2 of the 6 subjects, this effect was stronger when the component of frontal translation of the surface was vertical, rather than horizontal. No such anisotropy was found for the 4 other subjects. If a moving object is observed binocularly, the patterns of optic flow are different on the left and right retinae. We have suggested elsewhere (Cornilleau-Pérès & Droulez, in press) that this motion disparity might be used as avisual cue for the perception of a 3-D structure. Our model consisted in deriving binocular disparity from the left and right distributions of vertical velocities, rather than from luminous intensities, as has been done in classical studies on stereoscopic vision. The model led to some predictions concerning the detection of surface curvature from motion disparity in the presence or absence of intensity-based disparity (classically termedbinocular disparity). In a second set of experiments, we attempted to test these predictions, and we failed to validate our theoretical scheme from a physiological point of view. 相似文献
45.
Two visual half-field experiments tested Moscovitch’s (1979) proposition that cerebral asymmetry does not concern the earliest perceptual stages but only later processing. Subjects were briefly shown displays that included one (Experiment 1) or two (Experiment 2) types of forms differing in size and which, according to previous evidence, might lead to opposite laterality effects. Laterality effects were assessed for correct detections and for illusory conjunctions, both in terms of raw detection scores and in terms of perceptual discriminability (dr scores). In Experiment 1, displays included either rectangles or triangles. In the first case, the target was a cross; in the second case, it was a Star of David. A hemifield x size interaction was observed both on correct detections and on associated discriminability. Yet, no such interaction was obtained for illusory conjunctions or for associated d’ scores. In Experiment 2, the two types of forms were presented simultaneously, with the small ones either inside or outside the large ones. No laterality effects were observed. Some implications of these data for both hemispheric asymmetry and feature integration issues are discussed. The results suggest that early preattentive processes of feature extraction are not lateralized, whereas some integrative mechanisms, such as Treisman’s (1988) focal attention, may operate differently in the two hemispheres. 相似文献
46.
47.
Gerardo Marín Barbara V. Marín Eliseo J. Pérez-Stable Fabio Sabogal Regina Otero-Sabogal 《American journal of community psychology》1990,18(6):847-864
Tested a 7-month, media-based, community intervention among Hispanics in San Francisco designed to change levels of information on the damaging effects of cigarette smoking and on the availability of culturally appropriate cessation services. Three community-wide surveys of Hispanics were conducted with independent random samples, two as baselines (n = 1,660 and 2,053) and one postintervention (n = 1,965). Results showed that changes in the level of awareness of cessation services had taken place after implementation of the intervention. Furthermore, those changes took place primarily among the less acculturated Spanish-speaking Hispanics who were the target of the intervention. The changes in information reported here demonstrate that a culturally appropriate information dissemination campaign that utilizes multiple channels can produce changes in a community's level of information even when the campaign is implemented for a relatively short period. 相似文献
48.
Mart Fehér 《国际科学哲学研究》1990,4(3):229-240
The role accorded to the public by scientists and philosophers of science has undergone an essential historical change in the last three centuries. Public participation in (witnessing of) scientific experiments was considered an important requirement for 17th century experimenters (e.g. for Boyle or Pascal). The cognitive role played by lay persons was later substantially downgraded; witnessing went out of fashion, while science became more and more esoteric and a matter for experts only. Part of this process was that all scientific disciplines became intensively compartmentalized and in consequence a rather puzzling or even paradoxical situation appeared: that the scientists themselves were and are being reduced epistemically to the status of lay persons, outside of their proper field of expertise (as was pointed out by J. Hardwig). The paper deals with some cognitive aspects of this historical process. 相似文献
49.
Simon Davis 《Sex roles》1990,23(1-2):43-50
A study was made of 328 personal advertisements sampled from a major daily Canadian newspaper. It was found that gender differences for desired companion attributes were consistent with traditional sex role stereotypes. Relative to the opposite sex, women emphasized employment, financial, and intellectual status, as well as commitment, while men emphasized physical characteristics. Physical characteristics were the most desired, regardless of sex. Secondary findings were that, for this sample, considerably more men than women placed ads, and that the mean age for both sexes was relatively high. The main findings were similar to those from earlier studies.The author was formerly a psychiatric social worker employed by the Greater Vancouver Mental Health Service in Vancouver, B.C., Canada. He is presently a doctoral candidate at Simon Fraser University in Vancouver. The author would like to thank Dr. Ted Palys for his comments. 相似文献
50.