全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1495篇 |
免费 | 53篇 |
国内免费 | 2篇 |
专业分类
1550篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 18篇 |
2019年 | 36篇 |
2018年 | 25篇 |
2017年 | 34篇 |
2016年 | 32篇 |
2015年 | 27篇 |
2014年 | 40篇 |
2013年 | 163篇 |
2012年 | 51篇 |
2011年 | 57篇 |
2010年 | 49篇 |
2009年 | 35篇 |
2008年 | 49篇 |
2007年 | 53篇 |
2006年 | 50篇 |
2005年 | 51篇 |
2004年 | 43篇 |
2003年 | 40篇 |
2002年 | 50篇 |
2001年 | 29篇 |
2000年 | 28篇 |
1999年 | 26篇 |
1998年 | 15篇 |
1997年 | 20篇 |
1996年 | 16篇 |
1995年 | 26篇 |
1994年 | 13篇 |
1993年 | 18篇 |
1992年 | 17篇 |
1991年 | 15篇 |
1990年 | 15篇 |
1989年 | 14篇 |
1988年 | 19篇 |
1987年 | 18篇 |
1986年 | 12篇 |
1985年 | 22篇 |
1984年 | 21篇 |
1983年 | 14篇 |
1981年 | 12篇 |
1980年 | 15篇 |
1979年 | 19篇 |
1978年 | 13篇 |
1977年 | 18篇 |
1976年 | 19篇 |
1975年 | 18篇 |
1974年 | 19篇 |
1973年 | 15篇 |
1971年 | 14篇 |
1970年 | 14篇 |
1967年 | 17篇 |
排序方式: 共有1550条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
141.
142.
Direct and indirect aggression were studied in college students from China (women n=122; men n=97), a highly collectivistic culture; the US (women n=137; men n=136), a highly individualistic culture; and Poland (women n=105; men n=119), a culture with intermediate levels of collectivism and individualism. Consistent with a hypothesis derived from national differences in relative levels of collectivism and individualism, both direct and indirect aggression were higher in the US than in Poland and higher in Poland than in China. The theoretical implication of these results and directions for future research were discussed. 相似文献
143.
Sixty-five men and women, aged 50 to 88 years, completed Holland's Vocational Preference Inventory (VPI) and a work history questionnaire. Each participant was assigned one-letter Holland personality type codes for his or her first full-time job, longest full-time job, last job (if retired) or present job (if employed), and hobbies. In general, VPI scale scores and high point codes were consistent with the occupational codes but less consistent with the hobby code. The usefulness of Holland's vocational theory to the aged population is discussed. 相似文献
144.
145.
Anita M. Gordon 《Ethics & behavior》2014,24(3):175-194
University psychology and sociology researchers rated the likelihood they would engage in misconduct as described in 9 research scenarios, while also making moral judgments and rating the likelihood of discovery and sanctions. Multiple regression revealed significant effects across various scenarios for moral judgment as well as shame and embarrassment on reducing misconduct. The effects on misconduct of the perceived probability of sanctions were conditioned on moral judgments in some scenarios. The results have implications for how universities address the prevention, detection, and sanctioning of research misconduct. 相似文献
146.
147.
Is memory temporally organized? According to temporal distinctiveness models of memory, temporally isolated items should be better remembered than temporally crowded items in free recall tasks. Here, we tested this class of model by varying the temporal isolation of items either predictably (Experiment 1) or unpredictably (Experiment 2) in a free recall task. In both experiments, item recall probability increased as a function of the temporal gaps both before and after the item. The results are taken as support for temporal distinctiveness models of memory, in which items are represented and recalled in terms of their positions along a temporal dimension. 相似文献
148.
Logan GD 《Psychonomic bulletin & review》2006,13(1):139-144
Two experiments provided new measures of switch cost and retrieval time in the task span procedure. In Experiment 1, subjects
were given lists of six task names to remember, followed by six targets on which to perform the tasks named in the list. The
lists contained alternations and repetitions, and switch costs were estimated by comparing reaction time (RT) on alternation
and repetition trials. The experiment also included memory span and single task conditions, so switch costs could be estimated
by subtracting the sum of the RT in those conditions from the task span RT, as in the original report (Logan, 2004). The data
suggested that the original measure of switch cost was invalid and that the new measure was preferable. In Experiment 2, subjects
performed each task on the list twice. Retrieval was required on the first but not on the second trial in each pair. Retrieval
time was estimated by comparing the RT on trials that required retrieval with trials that did not require retrieval. This
measure was more valid than the RT in the memory span condition of Experiment 1, which was used in the original report. 相似文献
149.
150.
Duane Lundervold Lee Young Gordon Bourland Thomas Jackson 《Behavioral Interventions》1991,6(2):97-106
Internal consistency of two forms of the Geriatric Treatment Acceptability Survey (GTAS) and the Attitudes toward Behavior Analysis Scale (ABAS), a global measure of attitudes toward behavior modification, was evaluated. Alpha coefficients regarding treatment acceptability ratings for GTAS Form H were: (a) wandering 0.94; (b) hoarding 0.96; (c) disruptive behavior 0.92; (d) property destruction 0.95; and (e) aggression 0.94. Internal consistency for Form NH was: (a) wandering 0.96; (b) hoarding 0.97; (c) disruptive behavior 0.96; (d) property destruction 0.97; and (e) aggression 0.76. Internal consistency of the ABAS resulted in alpha coefficients of 0.90 (total score); 0.87 (ethics and humaneness); and 0.76 (use). These measures appear useful in assessing older adults, nursing home patients' and geriatric nursing staffs' global and specific attitudes regarding the use of behavioral treatments with older adults. Further research is needed to establish psychometric constructs assessed by the instruments and sensitivity to detecting change in attitudes based on educational or clinical interventions. 相似文献