全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1541篇 |
免费 | 29篇 |
专业分类
1570篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 23篇 |
2019年 | 36篇 |
2018年 | 26篇 |
2017年 | 34篇 |
2016年 | 33篇 |
2015年 | 28篇 |
2014年 | 39篇 |
2013年 | 163篇 |
2012年 | 51篇 |
2011年 | 57篇 |
2010年 | 49篇 |
2009年 | 35篇 |
2008年 | 49篇 |
2007年 | 54篇 |
2006年 | 50篇 |
2005年 | 51篇 |
2004年 | 43篇 |
2003年 | 40篇 |
2002年 | 50篇 |
2001年 | 29篇 |
2000年 | 29篇 |
1999年 | 26篇 |
1998年 | 16篇 |
1997年 | 20篇 |
1996年 | 16篇 |
1995年 | 26篇 |
1994年 | 13篇 |
1993年 | 18篇 |
1992年 | 17篇 |
1991年 | 15篇 |
1990年 | 15篇 |
1989年 | 14篇 |
1988年 | 19篇 |
1987年 | 18篇 |
1985年 | 22篇 |
1984年 | 22篇 |
1983年 | 15篇 |
1981年 | 12篇 |
1980年 | 18篇 |
1979年 | 19篇 |
1978年 | 13篇 |
1977年 | 18篇 |
1976年 | 19篇 |
1975年 | 18篇 |
1974年 | 20篇 |
1973年 | 15篇 |
1971年 | 14篇 |
1970年 | 14篇 |
1967年 | 17篇 |
1966年 | 12篇 |
排序方式: 共有1570条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
871.
The stimulus suffix effect (SSE) was examined with short sequences of words and meaningful nonspeech sounds. In agreement with previous findings, the SSE for word sequences was obtained with a speech, but not a nonspeech, suffix. The reverse was true for sounds. The results contribute further evidence for a functional distinction between speech and nonspeech processing mechanisms in auditory memory. 相似文献
872.
The movement-activated recording monitor (MARM) is a small noninvasive device that continuously detects, accumulates, and stores the amount of motor activity produced during sequential time intervals of designed duration. If motor activity totals are recorded every 15 min, the MARM is capable of calculating and storing 96 data points/day for 5.33 days. The MARM employs large-scale-integrated complementary-metal-oxide integrated circuits. To date, application has been primarily limited to psychiatric investigations, but the MARM is readily applicable for other time-motion studies of human or animal subjects. 相似文献
873.
Eugene C. Lechelt James Eliuk Gordon Tanne 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》1976,20(6):463-469
Stimulus orientation discrimination was investigated in visual and haptic modalities under conditions of simultaneous matching and memory. Discrimination of vertical and horizontal was significantly more accurate than discrimination of oblique stimulus orientations (45°, 135°, 225°, and 315°) for both modalities; haptic errors, however, were significantly greater at each orientation. While subjects were reliably more accurate in visually matching oblique stimulus orientations to a standard than producing them from memory, for the haptic modality, differences between memory and matching conditions were less consistent across the orientations sampled. 相似文献
874.
Clapper JP Bower GH 《Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition》2002,28(5):908-923
In 3 experiments, the authors provide evidence for a distinct category-invention process in unsupervised (discovery) learning and set forth a method for observing and investigating that process. In the 1st 2 experiments, the sequencing of unlabeled training instances strongly affected participants' ability to discover patterns (categories) across those instances. In the 3rd experiment, providing diagnostic labels helped participants discover categories and improved learning even for instance sequences that were unlearnable in the earlier experiments. These results are incompatible with models that assume that people learn by incrementally tracking correlations between individual features; instead, they suggest that learners in this study used expectation failure as a trigger to invent distinct categories to represent patterns in the stimuli. The results are explained in terms of J. R. Anderson's (1990, 1991) rational model of categorization, and extensions of this analysis for real-world learning are discussed. 相似文献
875.
Stewart N Brown GD 《Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition》2004,30(2):416-430
In contrast to exemplar and decision-bound categorization models, the memory and contrast models described here do not assume that long-term representations of stimulus magnitudes are available. Instead, stimuli are assumed to be categorized using only their differences from a few recent stimuli. To test this alternative, the authors examined sequential effects in a binary categorization of 10 tones varying in frequency. Stimuli up to 2 trials back in the sequence had a significant effect on the response to the current stimulus. The effects of previous stimuli interacted with one another. A memory and contrast model, according to which only ordinal information about the differences between the current stimulus and recent preceding stimuli is used, best accounted for these data. 相似文献
876.
A semi-persistent adult ocular dominance plasticity in visual cortex is stabilized by activated CREB
下载免费PDF全文

Pham TA Graham SJ Suzuki S Barco A Kandel ER Gordon B Lickey ME 《Learning & memory (Cold Spring Harbor, N.Y.)》2004,11(6):738-747
The adult cerebral cortex can adapt to environmental change. Using monocular deprivation as a paradigm, we find that rapid experience-dependent plasticity exists even in the mature primary visual cortex. However, adult cortical plasticity differs from developmental plasticity in two important ways. First, the effect of adult, but not juvenile monocular deprivation is strongly suppressed by administration of barbiturate just prior to recording visual evoked potentials, suggesting that the effect of adult experience can be inactivated acutely. Second, the effect of deprivation is less persistent over time in adults than in juveniles. This correlates with the known decline in CREB function during maturation of the visual cortex. To compensate for this decline in CREB function, we expressed persistently active VP16-CREB and find that it causes adult plasticity to become persistent. These results suggest that in development and adulthood, the regulation of a trans-synaptic signaling pathway controls the adaptive potential of cortical circuits. 相似文献
877.
Dovidio JF Gaertner SL Kawakami K Hodson G 《Cultural diversity & ethnic minority psychology》2002,8(2):88-102
The authors review a series of studies that illustrate how one form of contemporary racial bias of Whites, aversive racism, can shape different perspectives of Blacks and Whites in ways that can undermine race relations. This research demonstrates that contemporary racism among Whites is subtle, often unintentional, and unconscious but that its effects are systematically damaging to race relations by fostering miscommunication and distrust. In particular, the authors examine the effects of aversive racism on outcomes for Blacks (e.g., in selection decisions), on the ways that Whites behave in interracial interactions, in the impressions that Whites and Blacks form of each other in these interactions, and on the task efficiency of interracial dyads. 相似文献
878.
McAndrew FT Akande A Bridgstock R Mealey L Gordon SC Scheib JE Akande-Adetoun BE Odewale F Morakinyo A Nyahete P Mubvakure G 《The Journal of social psychology》2000,140(4):487-502
Citizens of 9 different English-speaking countries (N = 619) evaluated the average, or typical, citizen of 5 English-speaking countries (Great Britain, Canada, Nigeria, United States, Australia) on 9 pairs of bipolar adjectives. Participants were drawn from Australia, Botswana, Canada, Kenya, Nigeria, South Africa, the United States, Zambia, and Zimbabwe. There were statistically significant similarities in the rankings of the 5 stimulus countries on 8 of the 9 adjective dimensions and a strong convergence of autostereotypes and heterostereotypes on many traits. The results relate to previous stereotyping research and traditional methods of assessing the accuracy of national stereotypes. 相似文献
879.
Schachar R Mota VL Logan GD Tannock R Klim P 《Journal of abnormal child psychology》2000,28(3):227-235
The objective of this study was to determine whether deficient inhibitory control distinguishes children with a diagnosis of attention-deficit/hyperactivity (ADHD) disorder, conduct disorder (CD), and comorbid ADHD + CD from normally developing children. Participants were rigorously diagnosed children (age 7 to 12 years) with ADHD (N = 72), CD (N = 13) or ADHD + CD (N = 47) and 33 control children (NC). We studied inhibitory control using the stop-signal paradigm, a laboratory task that assessed the ability to inhibit an ongoing action. The ADHD group had significantly impaired inhibitory control compared to NC, CD, and ADHD + CD children. These results indicate that children with ADHD have deficient inhibition as measured in the stop-signal paradigm and that ADHD occurring in the presence of ADHD + CD may represent a phenocopy of CD rather than a variant of ADHD. 相似文献
880.
Gordon PC Hendrick R Foster KL 《Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition》2000,26(3):766-775
Experiments were performed using probe-word recognition methodology in which participants read sentences that were presented 1 word at a time and were then shown a probe word and had to make a speeded response indicating whether the word had occurred in the sentence. One experiment showed that response times to probe words increased with the size of the set of candidate probes. The other experiments showed that the effects caused by name repetition in circumstances in which the repeated name was co-referential also occurred when the repeated name was not co-referential and when the order of words in a sentence was scrambled. The results suggest that responses in the task can be based on probe-list memory, a mental representation created to keep track of those words that the participant believes are likely to be probed, and that the use of the task to make inferences about language comprehension should be accompanied by controls ruling out such strategies. 相似文献