全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1495篇 |
免费 | 53篇 |
国内免费 | 2篇 |
专业分类
1550篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 18篇 |
2019年 | 36篇 |
2018年 | 25篇 |
2017年 | 34篇 |
2016年 | 32篇 |
2015年 | 27篇 |
2014年 | 39篇 |
2013年 | 163篇 |
2012年 | 51篇 |
2011年 | 57篇 |
2010年 | 49篇 |
2009年 | 35篇 |
2008年 | 49篇 |
2007年 | 53篇 |
2006年 | 50篇 |
2005年 | 51篇 |
2004年 | 43篇 |
2003年 | 40篇 |
2002年 | 50篇 |
2001年 | 29篇 |
2000年 | 28篇 |
1999年 | 25篇 |
1998年 | 16篇 |
1997年 | 20篇 |
1996年 | 16篇 |
1995年 | 26篇 |
1994年 | 13篇 |
1993年 | 18篇 |
1992年 | 18篇 |
1991年 | 15篇 |
1990年 | 15篇 |
1989年 | 14篇 |
1988年 | 19篇 |
1987年 | 18篇 |
1986年 | 12篇 |
1985年 | 22篇 |
1984年 | 21篇 |
1983年 | 14篇 |
1981年 | 12篇 |
1980年 | 15篇 |
1979年 | 19篇 |
1978年 | 13篇 |
1977年 | 18篇 |
1976年 | 19篇 |
1975年 | 18篇 |
1974年 | 19篇 |
1973年 | 15篇 |
1971年 | 14篇 |
1970年 | 14篇 |
1967年 | 17篇 |
排序方式: 共有1550条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
51.
W.L. Marshall Janel Gauthier M.M. Christie D.W. Currie A. Gordon 《Behaviour research and therapy》1977,15(1):79-87
Three analogue experiments examined flooding therapy. Experiment 1 showed that flooding was more effective than standardized desensitization in reducing snake phobia. Experiment 2 examined three different modes of presenting the feared stimuli in flooding: taped auditory presentation, pictorial presentation, and a combination of these two. An additional combination group were given a brief in vivo exposure to the feared object immediately after each of three treatment sessions. Both a behavioral test and subjective estimates of fear showed advantages for the combined group that had the in vivo exposure, although it appeared that auditory instructions to imagine interaction with the snake was the best method for presenting the feared stimuli. Experiment 3 compared the auditory imagined method with and without ‘aversive’ or ‘implosive’ scenes, and with either an immediate or a delayed in vivo exposure. The only procedure to produce marked effects was the one that omitted ‘aversive’ scenes and provided immediate post-treatment in vivo exposure. 相似文献
52.
Raymond G. Romanczyk Daniel B. Crimmins William C. Gordon Wayne M. Kashinsky 《Behavior research methods》1977,9(4):393-394
While measurement of temperature as an index of circadian rhythmicity is not a difficult task with infrahuman subjects, long-term monitoring of deep body temperature with human subjects poses numerous measurement and safety problems. A preparation is described that is highly accurate, inexpensive, chronic, comfortable, and eliminates safety problems associated with more traditional methods of temperature measurement. 相似文献
53.
A total of 400 children at ages 5, 9, and 12 years were given a component selection task with stimuli differing in color and shape. Relative salience of these two stimulus components was manipulated across subjects in two ways: the standard shapes and colors from previous studies were compared with standard shapes in fluorescent colors, and the latter materials were compared with nonsense shapes in fluorescent colors. While fluorescence of color had little effect, the replacement of standard shapes with nonsense figures caused attention to be redirected from shape to color as the primary cue for learning. This effect was more pronounced at age 9 than at age 5. Apparently there is a greater tendency for older than for younger children to withdraw attention from a normally dominant component such as shape, when it is advantageous to adopt another feature such as color as the primary functional cue. An additional variable was integration of components (color within shape vs color as background for shape). Integration produced generally greater attention to color but did not affect the general pattern of salience effects. 相似文献
54.
Pitz GF 《Memory & cognition》1976,4(2):132-138
Two experiments examined the role of information storage and frequency estimation processes in a simple decision task. On each trial from one to five simultaneous sequences of information were presented, and subjects were required to monitor and respond to each sequence. In one study independent groups of subjects gave decisions, recognition responses. or frequency judgments. In a second study recall responses were obtained from subjects as part of the decision and frequency judgment tasks. Changes in performance as the number of simultaneous sequences increased suggested an independence of decisions from memory for specific information. Performance on the decision and frequency judgment tasks exceeded at times that which was possible on the basis of specific information available. Other data suggested that subjects in the frequency estimation tasks maintained an accurate frequency count when possible, but in the decision task kept little more than a record of their currently favored hypothesis. When direct counting was prevented in the second experiment. performance in the two tasks was not significantly different, suggesting that the two processes are very similar under such conditions. 相似文献
55.
56.
The development of a test to measure Elli's concept of rationality is described. In the first study discussed, a 58-item test is developed to measure rationality, and the reliability and convergent validity of the test is described. In a second study, the discriminant validity of the test is examined. An attempt is also made to reduce social-desirability content in the test by eliminating items most highly correlated with a Social Desirability Scale. The final 44-item test is found to be high in both reliability and validity. The factor structure of the test is also examined. 相似文献
57.
Gordon Hodson 《British journal of psychology (London, England : 1953)》2022,113(3):853-871
We overwhelmingly utilize (partially) informed consent for, and debriefing of, human research participants. Also common is the practice of reconsent, particularly where changes in study protocols (or in participants themselves) occur midstream – participants consent again to remaining in the project or to having their data included. Worryingly under-discussed is post-debriefing reconsent, wherein participants can withdraw their data after learning more fully of the study's goals and methods. Yet, major ethics bodies in Canada, the United States and the United Kingdom promote such practice, with vague and potentially problematic guidelines. Here, the author provides examples involving such reconsent practice, highlighting potentially serious problems that are scientific (e.g. threats to internal and external validity) and ethical (i.e. to the participant, their peers, the researcher and society) in nature. Particularly, problematic is the introduction, by design, of unknowable bias in our research findings. For example, highly prejudiced participants could withdraw data from a discrimination study after learning of the study's hypotheses and goals. The practice may arguably contradict an Open Science goal of increasing research transparency. This call for discussion about the direction of psychological science methods aims to engage a broader discussion in the research community. 相似文献
58.
59.
60.