全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1538篇 |
免费 | 29篇 |
专业分类
1567篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 21篇 |
2019年 | 36篇 |
2018年 | 25篇 |
2017年 | 34篇 |
2016年 | 33篇 |
2015年 | 28篇 |
2014年 | 39篇 |
2013年 | 163篇 |
2012年 | 51篇 |
2011年 | 57篇 |
2010年 | 49篇 |
2009年 | 35篇 |
2008年 | 49篇 |
2007年 | 54篇 |
2006年 | 50篇 |
2005年 | 51篇 |
2004年 | 43篇 |
2003年 | 40篇 |
2002年 | 50篇 |
2001年 | 29篇 |
2000年 | 29篇 |
1999年 | 26篇 |
1998年 | 16篇 |
1997年 | 20篇 |
1996年 | 16篇 |
1995年 | 26篇 |
1994年 | 13篇 |
1993年 | 18篇 |
1992年 | 17篇 |
1991年 | 15篇 |
1990年 | 15篇 |
1989年 | 14篇 |
1988年 | 19篇 |
1987年 | 18篇 |
1985年 | 22篇 |
1984年 | 22篇 |
1983年 | 15篇 |
1981年 | 12篇 |
1980年 | 18篇 |
1979年 | 19篇 |
1978年 | 13篇 |
1977年 | 18篇 |
1976年 | 19篇 |
1975年 | 18篇 |
1974年 | 20篇 |
1973年 | 15篇 |
1971年 | 14篇 |
1970年 | 14篇 |
1967年 | 17篇 |
1966年 | 12篇 |
排序方式: 共有1567条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
121.
Kirk R. Blankstein Gordon L. Flett Paul Boase Brenda B. Toner 《Anxiety, stress, and coping》2013,26(2):103-112
Abstract The primary purpose of the present study was to examine cognitive reactions of high and low test-anxious subjects while performing a task of extreme difficulty. A sample of 44 subjects (28 females, 16 males) attempted a difficult analogies task and then reported their thoughts during the task. Subjects also completed the Sarason Test Anxiety Scale and several other self-report measures of their cognitive reactions to the task. Thought listings were classified according to their referent (self, task, or unrelated) and their valence (positive, negative, or neutral). Correlational analyses were then performed with the various measures and revealed an association between test anxiety and reports of negative thoughts involving the self. Test anxiety was not related significantly to the number of thoughts involving the task nor to actual performance on the task. However, test-anxious subjects did report more negative expectancies for their performance and greater cognitive interference during the task. Negative self-expectancies were also related to more negative self thoughts, fewer positive self thoughts, and fewer positive task-related thoughts. The results are interpreted as further evidence for both the role of negative self-related cognitions in the experience of test anxiety and the usefulness of unstructured measures in anxiety research. 相似文献
122.
Gordon L. Flett Paul L. Hewitt Brenley Shapiro Jill Rayman 《Current psychology (New Brunswick, N.J.)》2001,20(4):289-311
Two studies examined the extent to which dimensions of perfectionism are associated with indices of relationship beliefs,
behaviors, and dyadic adjustment. In Study 1, 69 students in dating relationships completed the Multidimensional Perfectionism
Scale (MPS) and a multidimensional measure of relationship beliefs. In addition, partici-pants completed self-report measures
of positive and negative relationship behaviors, and global measures of liking and loving. In Study 2, 91 students in dating
relation-ships completed a battery of measures including the MPS and scales assessing perfec-tionism cognitions and perfectionistic
self presentation. They also completed mea-sures of dyadic adjustment and limerence (i.e., intense, obsessive low with fears
of rejection). Collectively, the findings indicated that individuals with high levels of self-oriented perfectionism and other-oriented
perfectionism have stronger relationship beliefs in the areas of communication, trust, and support, suggesting that these
perfec-tionists have high relationship standards in these particular areas. Although social prescribed perfectionism had little
association with specific relationship beliefs, so-cially prescribed perfectionism was associated with a tendency to display
destructive relationship responses (i.e., exit, neglect, and insensitivity), lower dyadic adjustment, and various aspects
of limerence, including obsessive preoccupations and emotional dependence on the dating partner. Perfectionistic self-presentation
and perfectionism cognitions were also linked with aspects of limerence. The results suggest that inter-personal aspects of
perfectionism are associated with self-defeating tendencies in dating relationships.
This research was supported, in part, by grant #410-93-1256 from the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada
awarded to the authors. The authors wish to thank Lisa Davidson for her assistance with the data analyses in Study 1. Requests
for reprints should be addressed to Gordon L. Flett, Department of Psychology, York University, 4700 Keele Street, Toronto,
Ontario, Canada, M3J 1P3. 相似文献
123.
Paul Gordon 《欧洲心理治疗、咨询与健康杂志》2013,15(3):493-495
Ivan Ward (ed.) (1997) The Presentation of Case Material in Clinical Discourse, London: Freud Museum Publications, 96 pp., $6.95 相似文献
124.
Ethical Theory and Moral Practice - 相似文献
125.
Recent temporal distinctiveness models of memory predict that temporally isolated items will be recalled better than temporally
crowded items. The effect has been found in some tasks (free recall, memory for serial order when report order is unconstrained,
running memory span) but not in others (forward serial recall). Such results suggest that the attentional weighting given
to a temporal dimension in memory may vary with task demands. Here, we find robust temporal isolation effects in recognition
memory (Experiment 1) and a smaller isolation effect in forward serial recall when an open pool of items is used (Experiment
2). Analysis of 26 temporal isolation effects suggests that the phenomenon occurs in a range of tasks but is larger when it
is useful to attend to a temporal dimension in memory. The overall pattern of results is taken to favor memory models that
rely on multiple weighted dimensions in memory, one of which is temporal. 相似文献
126.
Two experiments examined how lexical status affects the targeting of saccades during reading by using the boundary technique to vary independently the content of a letter string when seen in parafoveal preview and when directly fixated. Experiment 1 measured the skipping rate for a target word embedded in a sentence under three parafoveal preview conditions: full preview (e.g., brain–brain), pseudohomophone preview (e.g., brane–brain), and orthographic nonword control preview (e.g., brant–brain); in the first condition, the preview string was always an English word, while in the second and third conditions, it was always a nonword. Experiment 2 investigated three conditions where the preview string was always a word: full preview (e.g., beach–beach), homophone preview (e.g., beech–beach), and orthographic control preview (e.g., bench–beach). None of the letter string manipulations used to create the preview conditions in the experiments disrupted sublexical orthographic or phonological patterns. In Experiment 1, higher skipping rates were observed for the full (lexical) preview condition, which consisted of a word, than for the nonword preview conditions (pseudohomophone and orthographic control). In contrast, Experiment 2 showed no difference in skipping rates across the three types of lexical preview conditions (full, homophone, and orthographic control), although preview type did influence reading times. This pattern indicates that skipping not only depends on the presence of disrupted sublexical patterns of orthography or phonology, but also is critically dependent on processes that are sensitive to the lexical status of letter strings in the parafovea. 相似文献
127.
Gordon L. Flett Teresa Panico Paul L. Hewitt 《Current psychology (New Brunswick, N.J.)》2011,30(2):105-116
The current study examined the associations among dimensions of perfectionism, Type A behavior, self-efficacy, distress, and
health symptoms in high school students. A sample of 73 high school students (34 boys, 39 girls) completed measures of self-oriented
perfectionism, socially prescribed perfectionism, Type A behavior, self-efficacy, depressive symptoms, and psychosomatic symptoms.
Correlational analyses found that self-oriented perfectionism was marginally related to Type A behavior. Students with elevated
levels of depressive symptoms were also characterized by self-oriented perfectionism, Type A behavior, low self-efficacy,
and health symptoms. In addition, health symptoms were linked with low self-efficacy. Simultaneous entry of several variables
into a regression analysis found that significant unique predictors of depression were low self-efficacy and elevated self-oriented
perfectionism. Similarly, a regression analysis found that low self-efficacy and high self-oriented perfectionism were unique
predictors of health symptoms. The findings highlight the distinctions between the perfectionism and the Type A constructs,
and support self-regulation models of depression and physical symptoms that include an emphasis on excessive perfectionistic
standards and low self-efficacy. We discuss the need for preventive interventions designed for perfectionistic adolescents
with low self-efficacy. 相似文献
128.
We assessed intermanual transfer of the proprioceptive realignment aftereffects of prism adaptation in right-handers by examining alternate target pointing with the two hands for 40 successive trials, 20 with each hand. Adaptation for the right hand was not different as a function of exposure sequence order or postexposure test order, in contrast with adaptation for the left hand. Adaptation was greater for the left hand when the right hand started the alternate pointing than when the sequence of target-pointing movements started with the left hand. Also, the largest left-hand adaptation appeared when that hand was tested first after exposure. Terminal error during exposure varied in cycles for the two hands, converging on zero when the right hand led, but no difference appeared between the two hands when the left hand led. These results suggest that transfer of proprioceptive realignment occurs from the right to the left hand during both exposure and postexposure testing. Such transfer reflects the process of maintaining spatial alignment between the two hands. Normally, the left hand appears to be calibrated with the right-hand spatial map, and when the two hands are misaligned, the left-hand spatial map is realigned with the right-hand spatial map. 相似文献
129.
130.
In this study, we examined the hypothesis that semantic judgment tasks share overlapping processes if they require processing on common dimensions but not if they require processing on orthogonal dimensions in semantic space (Osgood, Suci, & Tannenbaum, 1957). We tested the hypothesis with the implicit association test (IATl Greenwald, McGhee, & Schwartz, 1998) in three experiments. Consistent with the hypothesis, IAT effects (costs in reaction time because of incompatible response mapping between associated judgment tasks) occurred consistently when judgment tasks tapped into common semantic dimensions, whereas no IAT effect appeared when judgment tasks entailed processing on orthogonal semantic dimensions. 相似文献