全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1204篇 |
免费 | 15篇 |
专业分类
1219篇 |
出版年
2019年 | 23篇 |
2018年 | 22篇 |
2017年 | 23篇 |
2016年 | 23篇 |
2015年 | 15篇 |
2014年 | 21篇 |
2013年 | 143篇 |
2012年 | 39篇 |
2011年 | 43篇 |
2010年 | 21篇 |
2009年 | 19篇 |
2008年 | 23篇 |
2007年 | 42篇 |
2006年 | 33篇 |
2005年 | 28篇 |
2004年 | 25篇 |
2003年 | 28篇 |
2002年 | 29篇 |
2001年 | 55篇 |
2000年 | 45篇 |
1999年 | 43篇 |
1998年 | 20篇 |
1997年 | 11篇 |
1995年 | 11篇 |
1994年 | 7篇 |
1993年 | 13篇 |
1992年 | 12篇 |
1991年 | 24篇 |
1990年 | 27篇 |
1989年 | 17篇 |
1988年 | 13篇 |
1987年 | 16篇 |
1986年 | 18篇 |
1985年 | 18篇 |
1984年 | 8篇 |
1983年 | 11篇 |
1982年 | 12篇 |
1980年 | 8篇 |
1979年 | 9篇 |
1978年 | 14篇 |
1976年 | 13篇 |
1975年 | 13篇 |
1974年 | 7篇 |
1973年 | 16篇 |
1972年 | 11篇 |
1970年 | 13篇 |
1969年 | 11篇 |
1968年 | 14篇 |
1967年 | 10篇 |
1966年 | 7篇 |
排序方式: 共有1219条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
231.
232.
Ceylan Z. Balaban Harun Karimpur Florian Röser Kai Hamburger 《Cognitive processing》2017,18(2):135-144
The present work investigated the impact of affect in landmark-based wayfinding. We assumed that affect-laden landmarks improve wayfinding performance and have an impact on later landmark recognition. To investigate our hypotheses, we ran two experiments in a virtual maze. In Experiment 1, we investigated how affect-laden landmarks influence wayfinding and recognition in comparison with neutral landmarks. The aim of Experiment 2 was to focus on the affective valence of a landmark. The memory tasks of both experiments were repeated after 1 week in order to assess memory consolidation. Results showed that the best wayfinding and recognition performance occurs when negatively laden landmarks were used. In comparison with neutral and positively laden landmarks, recognition performance hardly decreased over time for the negatively laden landmarks. Our results not only support findings in the field of emotion research but also expand the concept of semantic landmark salience with respect to emotional responses. 相似文献
233.
Robert E. Goodin 《The Journal of Ethics》2005,9(1-2):225-246
An interesting fact about customary international law is that the only way you can propose an amendment to it is by breaking it. How can that be differentiated from plain law-breaking? What moral standards might apply to that sort of international conduct? I propose we use ones analogous to the ordinary standards for distinguishing civil disobedients from ordinary law-breakers: would-be law-makers, like civil disobedients, must break the law openly; they must accept the legal consequences of doing so; and they must be prepared to have the same rules applied to them as everyone else. 相似文献
234.
235.
236.
CATHERINE Z. ELGIN 《Midwest Studies In Philosophy》2004,28(1):226-238
237.
238.
There is accumulating evidence that a variety of species possess quantitative abilities although their cognitive substrate
is still unclear. This study is the first to investigate whether sea lions (Otaria flavescens), in the absence of training, are able to assess and select the larger of two sets of quantities. In Experiment 1, the two
sets of quantities were presented simultaneously as whole sets, that is, the subjects could compare them directly. In Experiment
2, the two sets of quantities were presented item-by-item, and the totality of items was never visually available at the time
of choice. For each type of presentation, we analysed the effect of the ratio between quantities, the difference between quantities
and the total number of items presented. The results showed that (1) sea lions can make relative quantity judgments successfully
and (2) there is a predominant influence of the ratio between quantities on the subjects’ performance. The latter supports
the idea that an analogue representational mechanism is responsible for sea lions’ relative quantities judgments. These findings
are consistent with previous reports of relative quantities judgments in other species such as monkeys and apes and suggest
that sea lions might share a similar mechanism to compare and represent quantities. 相似文献
239.
Z. Seda Sahin David P. Nalbone Joseph L. Wetchler Jerry M. Bercik 《Contemporary Family Therapy》2010,32(3):238-256
The marriage and family therapy (MFT) field would benefit from new research about positive subjective experiences, such as
optimism, in order to construct and nurture strengths for developing more positive emotions. Through a better understanding
of optimism, marriage and family therapists (MFTs) can explore which strategies might help families and individuals better
cope with distressing emotions by building buffering strengths. In this study we address the relationship of differentiation,
family coping, and family functioning to optimism. We found that differentiation, family coping, and family functioning each
have a significant effect on attributional style. The results of this study also indicated that encouraging clients to take
an I-position and to reframe problems, in particular, may lead to greater optimism. This study contributes to the lack of
research regarding strength-based theories in the MFT field. 相似文献
240.
Janssen N Schirm W Mahon BZ Caramazza A 《Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition》2008,34(1):249-256
In 2 experiments participants named pictures of common objects with superimposed distractor words. In one naming condition, the pictures and words were presented simultaneously on every trial, and participants produced the target response immediately. In the other naming condition, the presentation of the picture preceded the presentation of the distractor by 1,000 ms, and participants delayed production of their naming response until distractor word presentation. Within each naming condition, the distractor words were either semantic category coordinates of the target pictures or unrelated. Orthogonal to this manipulation of semantic relatedness, the frequency of the pictures' names was manipulated. The authors observed semantic interference effects in both the immediate and delayed naming conditions but a frequency effect only in the immediate naming condition. These data indicate that semantic interference can be observed when target picture naming latencies do not reflect the bottleneck at the level of lexical selection. In the context of other findings from the picture-word interference paradigm, the authors interpret these data as supporting the view that the semantic interference effect arises at a postlexical level of processing. 相似文献